:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:多巴胺與經濟條件的操弄對大白鼠選擇行為的影響
作者:郭俊顯
作者(外文):Curler Chun-Hsien Kuo
校院名稱:國立中正大學
系所名稱:心理學研究所
指導教授:蕭世朗
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2002
主題關鍵詞:多巴胺行為經濟學需求曲線需求彈性經濟型態成本效益分析單位價格取代dopaminebehavioral economicsdemand curvedemand elasticityeconomy typecost-benefit analysisunit pricesubstitution
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(0) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:64
摘要
行為經濟學將個體經濟學引入動物行為的實驗分析之中,探討經濟條件與物品之間的交互作用如何影響動物行為。本論文利用行為經濟學的原理設計實驗,探討動物實驗中的成本與效益、經濟情境、所得水準如何影響動物行為。在實驗一中採用滑門設計配合不同成本與效益組合成的經濟情境,發現動物對糖水的的飲量與偏好會受到經濟情境的影響,且安非他命會促進糖水偏好。在實驗二中,在不同的經濟型態與所得水準測量動物對糖水的需求曲線,結果發現:所得水準的操弄沒有效果,糖水價格會影響糖水的需求彈性,經濟型態會影響多巴胺藥物是否表現效果。在閉鎖經濟型態中,安非他命會在低價格提高糖水偏好,但在高價格降低糖水偏好,多巴胺拮抗劑haloperidol在低價格提高糖水偏好,但在中至高價格降低偏好,且只在中等價格引發純水取代糖水的現象。但是經濟型態並未改變糖水的需求彈性,因此上述效果並非來自需求彈性的改變。在實驗三建立了快速測量動物的糖水需求曲線方法。在實驗四,利用快速需求曲線探討多巴胺D1與D2受體促進劑(SKF38393與bromocriptine)與拮抗劑(SCH23390與haloperidol)藥物的效果,結果發現只有D2受體的藥物會引發取代作用,但並未改變糖水的需求彈性,因此此種取代現象並非因需求彈性改變導致的。實驗五探討依核注射多巴胺D1受體拮抗劑SCH23390與D2受體拮抗劑haloperidol對快速需求曲線的影響,兩類藥物都降低糖水偏好,但同樣只有D2受體拮抗劑引發取代現象,此取代也並非糖水需求彈性改變引起的。在實驗六,比較了年齡對糖水偏好與多巴胺藥物的效果,老老鼠有較多的飲量與較低的糖水偏好,且安非他命降低老老鼠的糖水偏好,但提高年輕老鼠的糖水偏好。總結本論文的結果發現:價格、經濟型態與年齡這些經濟條件會影響多巴胺藥物效果的表現或與否與作用的方向。
關鍵詞:多巴胺、行為經濟學、滑門設計、需求曲線、經濟型態、所得水準、單位價格、需求彈性、SKF38393、SCH23390、bromocriptine、haloperidol、年齡、成本效益分析、偏好
Abstract
Behavioral economics uses the principles of microeconomics in the experimental analysis of animal behavior to investigate how economic factors and commodities interact to influence animal behaviors. In this study, the experiments were designed to understand the way cost and benefit factors, economy type and income level influence behaviors. In experiment 1, the sliding door design was used in the economic situations with different cost and benefit combinations. It was found that sucrose intake volume and preference was affected by these situations. Amphetamine facilitated sucrose preference in all situations. In experiment 2, demand curve of sucrose was measured in different economy type (closed or open) and income level. Results showed that income level did not affect either sucrose intake or preference. Unit price of sucrose changed the demand elasticity. Economy type affected whether the effect of dopaminergic agents was detected or not. In closed economy, amphetamine increased sucrose preference in low price situaions but decreased it in higher prices. Dopamine antagonist haloperidol also increased sucrose preference in low price, but decreased in medium to high prices. In addition, it induced water substitution in medium price. Since economy type did not change demand elasticity of sucrose, the above changes can not be attributed to the change of demand elasticity. In experiment 3, a method of rapid demand curve was developed. In experiment 4, the rapid demand curve was used to investigate the effects of systemic dopamine agonists, SKF3893 and bromicriptine, and antagonists, SCH23390 and haloperidol. Results indicated that only D2-specific agents induced water substitution, but they did not change demand elasticity of sucrose. In experiment 5, microinjection of either SCH23390 or haloperidol into nucleus accumbens decreased sucrose preference, but only haloperidol induced substitution. This substitution was not due to change of demand elasticity either. In experiment 6, sucrose preference and effect of dopaminergic agents on preference in young and aged rats were compared. Aged rats drank more sucrose and had less sucrose preference. Amphetamine had different effects in two groups, the drug decreased preference in aged rats but increased it in young rats. In conclusion, the effect of dopamine agents was dependent on unit price of commodity, economy type and age.
Keywords: Dopamine, Behavioral Economics, Sliding Door Design, Demand Curve, Economy Type, Income Level, Unit Price, Demand Elasticity, SKF38393, SCH23390, bromocritpine, haloperidol, age, Cost-Benefit Analysis, Preference
參考文獻
Aberman, J.E., & Salamone, J.D. (1999). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion make rats more sensitive to high ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement. Neuroscience, 92(2), 545-552.
Aberman, J.E., Ward, S.J., & Salamone, J.D. (1998). Effects of dopamine antagonists and accumbends dopamine depletions on time-constrained progressive-ratio performance. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 61, 341-348.
Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463-496.
Ainslie, G. (1992). Temporary-preference theory. In Anonymous, Picoeconomics: The strategic interaction of successive motivational states within the person. (pp. 56-95). New York: Cambridge Universtiy Press.
Bailey, C.S., Hsiao, S., & King, J.E. (1986). Hedonic reactivity to sucrose in rats: modification by pimozide. Physiology & Behavior, 38, 447-452.
Baum, W.M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 231-242.
Baum, W.M. (1979). Matching, undermatching, and overmatching in the studies of choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 32, 269-281.
Baum, W.M., & Rachlin, H. (1969). Choice as time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 861-874.
Bauman, R. (1991). An experimental analysis of the cost of food in a closed economy. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 33-50.
Bauman, R.A., Raslear, T.G., Hursh, S.R., Shurtleff, D., & Simmons, L. (1996). Substitution and Caloric Regulation in a Closed Economy. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65(2), 401-422.
Berridge, K.C. (1996). Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20(1), 1-25.
Bickel, W.K., DeGrandpre, R.J., & Higgins, S.T. (1993). Behavioral economics: a novel experimental approach to the study of drug dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 33, 173-192.
Bickel, W.K., DeGrandpre, R.J., & Higgins, S.T. (1995). The behavioral economics of concurrent drug reinforcers: a review and reanalysis of drug self-administration research. Psychopharmacology, 118, 250-259.
Bickel, W.K., DeGrandpre, R.J., Higgins, S.T., & Hughes, J.R. (1990). Behavioral economics of drug self-administration. I. Functional equivalence of response requirment and drug dose. Life Sciences, 47, 1501-1510.
Bickel, W.K., DeGrandpre, R.J., Hughes, J.R., & Higgins, S.T. (1991). Behavioral economics of drug self-administration: II. A unit-price analysis of cigarette smoking. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 145-154.
Bickel, W.K., Green, L., & Vuchinich, R.E. (1995). Behavioral Economics. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(3), 257-262.
Blackburn, J.R., Phillips, A.G., & Fibiger, H.C. (1987). Dopamine and Preparatory Behavior: I. Effects of Pimozide. Behavioral Neuroscience, 101(3), 352-360.
Cabanac, M. (1992). Pleasure:the common currency. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 155, 173-200.
Campbell, U.C., Rodefer, J.S., & Carroll, M.E. (1999). Effects of dopamine receptor antagonists (D1and D2) on the demand for smoked cocaine base in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology, 144, 381-388.
Carroll, M.E. (1985). Concurrent phencyclidine and saccharin access: presentation of an alternative reinforcer reduces drug intake. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 131-144.
Carroll, M.E. (1987a). Concurrent access to two concentrations of orally delivered phencyclidine: Effects of feeding conditions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 347-362.
Carroll, M.E. (1987b). Self-administration of orally-delivered phencyclidine and ethanol under concurrent fixed-ratio schedules in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology, 93, 1-7.
Carroll, M.E. (1993). The economic context of drug and non-drug reinforcers affects acquisition and maintenance of drug-reinforced behavior and withdrawal effects. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 33 , 201-210.
Carroll, M.E. (1998). Income alters the relative reinforcing effects of drug and nondrug reinforcers. National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series, 6407
Carroll, M.E., & Carmona, G.G. (1991a). Effects of food FR and food deprivation on disruptions in food-maintained performance of monkeys during phencyclidine withdrawal. Psychopharmacology, 104, 143-149.
Carroll, M.E., Carmona, G.G., & May, S.A. (1991b). Modifying drug-reinforced behavior by altering the economic conditions of the drug and a nondrug reinforcer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 361-376.
Carroll, M.E., Lac, S.T., & Nygaard, S.L. (1989). A concurrently available nondrug reinforcer prevents the acquisition or decreases the maintenance of cocaine-reinforced behavior. Psychopharmacology, 97, 23-29.
Carroll, M.E., & Meisch, R.A. (1984a). Increased drug-reinforced behavior due to food deprivation. Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology, 4, 47-88.
Carroll, M.E., & Stotz, D.C. (1984b). Increased phencyclidine self-administration due to food deprivation: interaction with concentration and training conditions. Psychopharmacology, 84, 299-303.
Caul, W. F. & Brindle, N. A.(2001) Schedule-dependent effects of haloperidol and amphetamine: multiple-schedule task shows within-subject effects. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 68, 53-63.
Clifton, P.G., Vickers, S.P., & Somerville, E.M. (1998). Little and often: ingestive behavior patterns following hippocampal lesions in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(3), 502-511.
Collier, G. (1989). The economics of hunger, thirst, satiety, and regulation. The Psychobiology of Human Eating Disorders: Preclinical and Clinical Perspectives, 575, 136-154.
Collier, G., Johnson, D.F., & Berman, J. (1998). Patch choice as a function of procurement cost and encounter rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69(1), 5-16.
Collier, G., Johnson, D.F., & Morgan, C. (1992). The Magnitude-of-Reinforcement Function in Closed and Open Economies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 81-89.
Collier, G., Johnson, D.F., & Morgan, C. (1997). Meal patterns of cats encountering variable food procurement cost. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 303-310.
Collier, G.H. (1983). Life in a closed economy: The ecology of learning and motivation. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in Analysis of Behavior. (pp. 223-274). John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Collier, G.H. (1985). Satiety: An ecological perspective. Brain Research Bulletin, 14, 693-700.
Collier, G.H., Johnson, D.F., Cybulski, K.A., & McHale, C.A. (1990). Activity patterns in rats(Rattus norvegicus) as a function of the cost of access to four resources. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104(1), 53-65.
Collier, G.H., Johnson, D.F., Hill, W.L., & Kaufman, L.W. (1986). The economics of the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 113-136.
Conover, K.L., & Shizgal, P. (1994a). Competition and sumation between rewarding effects of sucrose and lateral hypothalamic stimulation in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(3), 537-548.
Conover, K.L., & Shizgal, P. (1994b). Differential effects of postingestive feedback on the reward value of sucrose and lateral hypothalamic stimulation in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(3), 559-572.
Conover, K.L., Woodside, B., & Shizgal, P. (1994c). Effects of sodium depletion on competition and summation between rewarding effects of salt and lateral hypothalamic stimulation in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(3), 549-558.
Cooper, S.J., & Francis, J. (1993). A Microstructural Analysis of the Effects of Presatiation on Feeding Behavior in the Rat. Physiology &.Behavior, 33, 413-416.
Cousins, M.S., Atherton, A., Turner, L., & Salamone, J.D. (1996). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions alter relative response allocation in a T-maze cost/benefit task. Behavioural Brain Research, 74, 189-197.
Cousins, M.S., & Salamone, J.D. (1994). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions in rats affect relative response allocation in a novel cost/benefit procedure. Pharmacology Biochemistry &.Behavior, 49(1), 85-91.
Cousins, M.S., Sokolowski, J.D., & Salamone, J.D. (1993). Different effects of nucleus accumbens and ventrolateral striatal dopamine depletion on instrumental response selection in the rat. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 46, 943-951.
Cousins, M.S., Wei, W., & Salamone, J.D. (1994). Pharmacological characterization of performance on a concurrent lever pressing/feeding choice procedure: effects of dopamine antagonist, cholinomimetic, sedative and stimulant drugs. Psychopharmacology, 116, 529-537.
Das, S., & Fowler, S.C. (1995). Acute and subchronic effects of clozapine on licking in rats: tolerance to disruptive effects on number of licks, but no tolerance to rhythm slowing. Psychopharmacology, 120, 249-255.
DeGrandpre, R.J., Bickel, W.K., Hughes, J.R., Layng, M.P., & Badger, G. (1993a). Unit price as a useful metric in analyzing effects of reinforcer magnitude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 641-666.
DeGrandpre, R.J., Bickel, W.K., Rizvi, S.A.T., & Hughes, J.R. (1993b). Effects of income on drug choice in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59(3), 483-500.
Elsmore, T.F., Fletcher, G.V., Conrad, D.G., & Sodetz, F.J. (1980). Reduction of heroin intake in boboons by an economic constraint. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 13(5), 729-731.
English, J.A., Rowlett, J.K., & Woolverton, W.L. (1995). Unit-Price Analysis of Opioid Consumption by Monkeys Responding under a Progressive-Ratio Schedule of Drug Injection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(3), 361-371.
Ettenberg, A., & Camp, C.H. (1986). Haloperidol induces a partial reinforcement extinction effect in rats: Implications for a dopamine involvement in food reward. Pharmacology Biochemistry &.Behavior, 25, 813-821.
Ettenberg, A., & Horvitz, J.C. (1990). Pimozide Prevents the Response-Reinstating Effects of Water Reinforcement in Rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry &.Behavior, 37, 465-469.
Ettenberg, A., Koob, G.F., & Bloom, F.E. (1981). Response artifact in the measurement of neuroleptic-induced anhedonia. Science, 213(4505), 357-359.
Evenden, J.L., & Robbins, T.W. (1983). Dissociable effects of d-amphetamine, chlordiazepoxide, and alpha-flupenthixol on choice and rate measures of reinforcement in the rat. Psychopharmacology, 79, 180-186.
Fantino, E., & Preston, R.A. (1988). Choice and foraging: The effects of accessibility on acceptability. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50(3), 395-403.
Fautsman, W.O., & Fowler, S.C. (1981). Use of operant response duration to distinguish effects of haloperidol from non-reward. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 15, 327-329.
Foltin, R.W., & Evans, S.M. (1999). The effects of d-amphetamine on intake of food and a sweet fluid containing cocaine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 62(3), 457-464.
Fouriezos, G., & Wise, R.A. (1976). pimozide-induced extinction of intracranial self-stimulation: response patterns rule out motor or performance deficits. Brain Research, 103, 377-380.
Fowler, S.C. (1990). Neuroleptics produce within-session response decrements: Facts and Therories. Drug Development Research, 20, 101-116.
Fowler, S.C., & Das, S. (1994). Haloperidol-induced decrements in force and duration of rats' tongue movements during licking are attenuated by concomitant anticholinergic treatment. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 49, 813-817.
Fowler, S.C., Gramling, S.E., & Liao, R.M. (1986). Effects of pimozide on emitted force, duration and rate of operant response maintained at low and high levels of required force. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 25, 615-622.
Fowler, S.C., & Mortell, C. (1992). Low doses of haloperidol interfere with rat tongue extensions during licking: a quantitative analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 386-395.
Fowler, S.C., & Senyuz, L. (1993). Effects of haloperidol on a run-climb-run behavioral task: distance climbed does not alter within-session decrements. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107(4), 651-661.
Gramling, S.E., Fowler, S.C., & Collins, K.R. (1984). Some effects of pimozide on nondeprived rats licking sucrose solutions in an anhedonia paradigm. Pharmacology Biochemistry &.Behavior, 21, 617-624.
Green, L., & Freed, D.E. (1993). The substitutability of reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 141-158.
Green, L., Fry, A.F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: a life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33-36.
Green, L., & Rachlin, H. (1975). Economic and biological influences on a pigeon's key peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 55-62.
Green, L., & Rachlin, H. (1991). Economic substitutability of electrical brain stimulation, food , and water. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 133-143.
Hamill, S., Trevitt, J.T., Nowend, K.L., Carlson, B.B., & Salamone, J.D. (1999). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions and time-constrained progressive ratio performance: effects of different ratio requirements. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 64(1), 21-27.
Hastjarjo, T., & Silberberg, A. (1992). Effects of reinforcer delays on choice as a function of income level. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 119-125.
Hastjarjo, T., Silberberg, A., & Hursh, S.R. (1990a). Quinine pellets as an inferior good and a Giffen good in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 263-271.
Hastjarjo, T., Silberberg, A., & Hursh, S.R. (1990b). Risky choice as a function of amount and variance in food supply. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 155-161.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 267-272.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1981). A first law for behavioral analysis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 392-395.
Herrnstein, R.J. (1990). Rational choice theory: Necessary but not sufficient. American Psychologist, 45, 356-367.
Herrnstein, R.J., & Prelec, D. (1991). Melioration: A theory of distributed choice. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5, 137-156.
Herrnstein, R.J., & Vaughan, W. (1980). Melioration and behavioral allocation. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: the allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Horvitz, J.C., & Ettenberg, A. (1989). Haloperidol blocks the respose-reinstating effects of food reward: A methodology for separating neuroleptic effects on reinforcement and motor processes. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 31, 861-865.
Hsiao, S., & Smith, G.P. (1995). Raclopride reduces sucrose preference in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 50(1), 121-125.
Hursh, S.R. (1978). The economics of daily consumption controlling food- and water-reinforced responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 475-491.
Hursh, S.R. (1980). Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 219-238.
Hursh, S.R. (1991). Behavioral economics of drug self-administration and drug abuse policy. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 377-393.
Hursh, S.R. (2000). Behavioral economics concepts and methods for studying health behavior. In W. K. Bickel & R. E. Vuchinich (Eds.), Reframing health behavior change with behavioral economics. (pp. 27-60). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hursh, S.R., & Natelson, B.H. (1981). Electrical brain stimulation and food reinforcement dissociated by demand elasticity. Physiology & Behavior, 26, 509-515.
Hursh, S.R., Raslear, T.G., Bauman, R., & Black, H. (1989). The quantitative analysis of economic behavior with laboratory animals. In G. G. Klaus & F. Olander (Eds.), Understanding economic behaviour. (pp. 393-407). The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Hursh, S.R., Raslear, T.G., Shurtleff, D., Bauman, R., & Simmons, L. (1988). A cost-benefit analysis of demand for food. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 419-440.
Hursh, S.R., & Winger, G. (1995). Normalized Demand for Drugs and Other Reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(3), 373-384.
Johnson, D.F., & Collier, G. (1994). Meal patterns of rats encountering variable food procurement cost. Animal Behavior, 47, 1279-1287.
Johnson, D.F., Triblehorn, J., & Collier, G. (1993). The effect of patch depletion on meal patterns in rats. Animal Behavior, 46, 55-62.
Johnson, D.F., Triblehorn, J., & Collier, G. (1995). Rat's meal-ending rules in depleting patches. Animal Behavior, 49, 1707-1709.
Kagel, J.H., Dwyer, J.G.P., & Battalio, R.C. (1985). Bliss point vs. minimum needs: tests of competing motivational processes. Behavioural Processes, 11, 61-77.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision uder risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291.
Kahneman, D., Wakker, P.P., & Sarin, R. (1997). Back to Bentham? Explorations of experienced utility. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 375-405.
Kerwin, M.E., Ahearn, W.H., Eicher, P.S., & Burd, D.M. (1995). The cost of eating: a behavioral economic analysis of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 245-260.
Kirkpatrick, M.A., & Fowler, S.C. (1989). Force-proportional reinforcement: pimozide does not reduce rats' emission of higher forces for sweeter reward. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 32, 499-504.
Koch, M., Schmid, A., & Schnitzler, H.-U. (2000). Role of nucleus accumbens dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in instrumental and Pavlovian paradigms of conditioned reward. Psychopharmacology, 152, 67-73.
Le Moal, M., & Simon, H. (1991). Mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic network: functional and regulatory roles. Physiological Reviews, 71, 155-234.
Leibowitz, S.F., Alexander, J.T., Cheung, W.K., & Weiss, G.F. (1993). Effects of serotonin and the serotonin blocker metergoline on meal patterns and macronutrient selection. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 45, 185-194.
Liao, R.M., & Fowler, S.C. (1990). Haloperidol produces within-session increments in operant response duration in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 36, 199-201.
Logue, A.W. (1986). Choice. In A. W. Logue (Ed.), The Psychology of Eating and Drinking. (pp. 103-123). New York: W.H. Freeman and Company.
Martin-Iverson, M.T., Wilke, & Fibiger, H.C. (1987). Effect of haloperidol and amphetamine on percieved quality of food and tones. Psychopharmacology, 93, 374-381.
Mazur, J.E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: vol. 5 The effect of dely and of intervening events on reinforcement value. (pp. 55-73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mazur, J.E. (1996). Past experience, recency, and spontaneous recovery in choice behavior. Animal Learning &.Behavior, 24(1), 1-10.
Mazur, J.E., Snyderman, M., & Coe, D. (1985). Influences of delay and rate of reinforcement on discrete trial choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 565-575.
McCullough, L.D., Cousins, M.S., & Salamone, J.D. (1993). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in responding on a continuous reinforcement operant schedule: A neurochemical and behavioral study. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 46, 581-586.
McFarland, D., & Ettenberg, A. (1999). Haloperidol does not attenuate conditioned place preferences or locomotor activation produced by food- or heroin-predictive discriminative cues. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 62(4), 631-641.
McFarland, K., & Ettenberg, A. (1995). Haloperidol differentially affects reinforcement and motivation processes in rats running an alley for intravenous heroin. Psychopharmacology, 122, 346-350.
McFarland, K., & Ettenberg, A. (1998). Haloperidol does not affect motivational processes in an operant runway model of food-seeking behavior. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(3), 630-635.
Miller, H.L. (1976). Matching-based hedonic scaling in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 335-347.
Mitchell, S.H., & Brener, J. (1997). The work costs of earning food as a determinant of patch leaving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23(2), 136-144.
Morato, S., Johnson, D.F., & Collier, G. (1995). Feeding patterns of rats when food-access cost is alternately low and high. Physiology &.Behavior, 57(1), 21-26.
Myerson, J., & Green, L. (1995). Discounting of Delayed Reward: Models of Individual Choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(3), 263-276.
Nader, M.A., Hedeker, D., & Woolverton, W.L. (1993). Behavioral economics and drug choice: effects of unit price on cocaine self-administration by monkeys. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 33, 193-199.
Navin, J.A. (1969). Interval reinforcement of choice behavior in discrete trials. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 875-885.
Navin, J.A. (1979). Overall matching versus momentary maximizing : Navin(1969) revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 300-306.
Nowend, K.L., Arizzi, M., Carlson, B.B., & Salamone, J.D. (2001). D1 or D2 antagonism in nucleus accumbens core or dorsomedial shell suppresses lever pressing for food but leads to compensatory increases in chow consumption. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 69, 373-382.
Rachlin, H. (1970). Introdution to Modern Behaviorism. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman & Company Publishers.
Rachlin, H. (1971). On the tautology of matching law. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 249-251.
Rachlin, H. (1974). Self-control. Behaviorism, 2, 94-107.
Rachlin, H. (1989). Reinforcement and Punishment. In H. Rachlin (Ed.), Judgement, decision, and choice: A cognitive/behavioral synthesis. (pp. 115-146). New York: W.H. Freeman and Company.
Rachlin, H. (1991). Self control. In R. Howard (Ed.), Introduction to Modern Behaviorism. (pp. 264-285). San Francisco: W.H. Freeman & Company Publishers.
Rachlin, H. (1995). Behavioral economics without anomalies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 397-404.
Rachlin, H., Battalio, R.C., Kagel, J., & Green, L. (1981). Maximization theory in the behavioral psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 371-417.
Rachlin, H., & Green, L. (1972). Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 15-22.
Rachlin, H., Green, L., Kagel, J., & Battalio, R.C. (1976). Economic demand theory and psychological studies of choice. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. (pp. 129-154).
Rachlin, H., Logue, A.W., Gibbon, J., & Frankel, M. (1986). Cognition and behavior in studies of choice. Psychogical Review, 93, 33-45.
Rachlin, H., Raineri, A., & Cross, D. (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 233-244.
Raslear, T.G., Bauman, R.A., Hursh, S.R., Shurtleff, D., & Simmons, L. (1988). Rapid demand curves for behavioral economics. Animal Learning &.Behavior, 16(3), 330-339.
Rodefer, J.S., & Carroll, M.E. (1996). Progressive ratio and behavioral economic evaluation of the reinforcing efficacy of orally delivered phencyclidine and ethanol in monkeys: effects of feeding conditions. Psychopharmacology, 128, 265-273.
Rodefer, J.S., & Carroll, M.E. (1997). A comparison of progressive ratio schedules versus behavioral economic measures: effect of an alternative reinforcer on the reinforcing efficacy of phencyclidine. Psychopharmacology, 132, 95-103.
Rodefer, J.S., & Carroll, M.E. (1999). Concurrent progressive-ratio schedules to compare reinforceing effectiveness of different phencyclidine (PCP) concentrations in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology, 144, 163-174.
Rodefer, J.S., DeRoche, K.K., Lynch, W.A., & Carroll, M.E. (1996). Feeding conditions alter the demand for phencyclidine and ethanol: A behavioral economic analysis. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4(1), 61-67.
Rovee-Collier, C.K., Clapp, B.A., & Collier, G.H. (1982). The Economics of Food Choice in Chicks. Physiology &.Behavior, 28, 1097-1102.
Salamone, J.D. (1991). Behavioral pharmacology of dopamine systems: a new synthesis. In P. Willner & J. Scheel-Kruger (Eds.), The mesolimbic dopamine system: from motivation to action. (pp. 599-613). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Salamone, J.D. (1992). Complex motor and sensorimotor functions of striatal and accumbens dopamine: involvement in instrumental behavior processes. Psychopharmacology, 107, 160-174.
Salamone, J.D., Cousins, M.S., McCullough, L.D., Carriero, D.L., & Berkowitz, R.J. (1994). Nucleus accumbens dopamine release increases during instrumental lever pressing for food but not free food consumption. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 49(1), 25-31.
Salamone, J.D., Cousins, M.S., & Snyder, B.J. (1997). Behavioral Functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine: Empirical and conceptual problems with the anhedonia hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 21(3), 341-359.
Salamone, J.D., Kurth, P.A., McCullough, L.D., & Sokolowski, J.D. (1995). The effects of nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions on continuously reinforced operant responding: contrasts with the effects of extinction. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 50(3), 437-443.
Salamone, J.D., Kurth, P.A., McCullough, L.D., Sokolowski, J.D., & Cousins, M.S. (1993a). The role of brain dopamine in response initiation: effects of haloperidol and regionally specific dopamine depletions on the local rate of instrumental responding. Brain Research, 628, 218-226.
Salamone, J.D., Mahan, K., & Rogers, S. (1993b). Ventrolateral striatal dopamine depletions impair feeding and food handling in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry &.Behavior, 44, 605-610.
Salamone, J.D., Steinpreis, R.E., McCullough, L.D., Smith, P., Grebel, D., & Mahan, K. (1991). Halperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion suppress lever pressing for food but increase free food consumption in a novel food choice procedure. Psychopharmacology, 104, 515-521.
Schneider, L.H., Davis, J.D., Watson, C.A., & Smith, G.P. (1990). Similar effect of raclopride and reduced sucrose concentration on the microstructure of sucrose sham feeding. European Journal of Pharmacology, 186, 61-70.
Senyuz, L., & Fowler, S.C. (1993). Raclopride, but not SCH23390, induces maldirected jumping in rats trained to perform a run-climb-run behavioral task. Psychopharmacology, 112, 518-520.
Shurtleff, D., Warren-Boulton, F.R., & Silberberg, A. (1987). Income and choice between different goods. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 263-275.
Siegel, E., & Rachlin, H. (1995). Soft Commitment: Self-Control Achieved by Response Persistence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(2), 117-128.
Silberberg, A., Murray, P., Christensen, J., & Asano, T. (1988). Choice in teh repeated-gambles experiment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 187-195.
Silberberg, A., Warren-Boulton, F.R., & Asano, T. (1987). Inferior-good and Giffen-good effects in monkey choice behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 13(3), 292-301.
Sokolowski, J.D., & Salamone, J.D. (1998). The role of accumbens dopamine in lever pressing and response allocation: Effects of 6-OHDA injected into core and dorsomedial shell. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 59(3), 557-566.
Staddon, J.E.R., & Hinson, J.M. (1983). Optimization: a result of a mechanism? Science, 221, 976-977.
Steinpreis, R.E., Baskin, P., & Salamone, J.D. (1993a). Vacuous jaw movements induced by sub-chronic administration of haloperidol: interaction with scopolamine. Psychopharmacology, 111, 99-105.
Steinpreis, R.E., & Salamone, J.D. (1993b). Effects of acute haloperidol and reserpine administration on vacuous jaw movements in three different age groups of rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 46, 405-409.
Tombaugh, T.N., Anisman, H., & Tombaugh, J. (1980). Extinction and dopamine receptor blockade after intermittent reinforcement training: Failure to observe functional equivalence. Psychopharmacology, 70, 19-28.
Towell, A., Muscat, R., & Willner, P. (1987). Effects of pimozide on sucrose consumption and preference. Psychopharmacology, 92, 262-264.
Trevitt, J.T., Carlson, B.B., Nowend, K., & Salamone, J.D. (2001). Substantia nigra pars reticulata is a highly potent site of action for the behavioral effects of the D1 antagonist SCH23390 in the rat. Psychopharmacology, 156, 32-41.
Tsunematsu, S. (2000). Effort- and time-cost effects on demand curves for food by pigeons under short session close economies. Behavioural Processes, 53, 47-56.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453-458.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss aversion in riskess choice: A reference-dependent model. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 1039-1061.
Willner, P., Chawla, K., Sampson, D., Sophokleous, S., & Muscat, R. (1988). Tests of functional equivalence between pimozide pretreatment, extinction and free feeding. Psychopharmacology, 95, 423-426.
Wise, R.A., Spindler, J., de Wit, H., & Gerber, G.J. (1978). Neuroleptic-induced "anhedonia" in rats: pimozide blocks reward quality of food. Science, 201, 262-264.
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
:::
無相關博士論文
 
無相關書籍
 
無相關著作
 
無相關點閱
 
QR Code
QRCODE