:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:單手截取行動時間與空間的適應:直接知覺觀點
作者:彭國威
作者(外文):Wayne Gwo-wei Peng
校院名稱:國立臺灣師範大學
系所名稱:體育學系
指導教授:卓俊伶
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2011
主題關鍵詞:單手截取行動直接知覺身體尺度訊息環境賦使適應one-handed interceptive actiondirect perceptionbody-scaled informationaffordancesadaptation
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(0) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:28
行動者必須精確地調整時間與空間的參數,以執行日常活動與運動表現的截取行動。直接知覺概念主張個體直接擷取身體尺度訊息引致行動,並與環境交互作用;行動的適應是協調與控制改變的過程,受有機體、環境與工作之限制。本研究旨在探討不同環境條件對單手截取行動之時間、空間參數與內在變項的影響,並檢驗行動尺度比率的界限與動作適應。30名成人以單手分別截取5種不同速度(2.1、3.2、4.6、5.4與6.0 公尺 / 秒)球體,並以平衡消去安排球速,每種球速試作10次。依變項蒐集9項運動學參數(截取行動的拇—中指最大開口距離、拇—中指開口最大速度、潛伏時間、傳送階段時間與位移、抓握階段時間與位移、接觸前最大速度、抓握最大速度)與3項速度相關行動尺度換算參數。經重複量數單因子變異數分析發現:(一)單手截取較快速度來球,手部最大開口距離增加,且手部最大開口距離的變異性較為穩定;(二)單手截取較快速度來球,動作速度變快,其相對潛伏時間與動作時間較短;(三)單手截取較快速度來球,調整向前位移距離,其傳送階段位移與抓握階段位移較短;與(四)單手截取較快速度來球行動尺度比率較大,且改變達特定閾值,行動尺度比率維持特定範圍。
One has to precisely attune temporal and spatial parameters for interceptive actions in daily activities and sports performance. Direct perception concepts proposed individual’s action is led by directly picking up body-scaled information and interacts with environment. Adaptation of action is the process of changing coordination and control that was constrained by organism, environment, and task. This study investigated influence of environmental conditions on temporal parameters, spatial parameters, and intrinsic measures with one-handed interceptive action, and examined the boundary of action-scaled ratios and movement adaptation. Thirty adults were required to use one hand to intercept ball with 5 speeds (2.1, 3.2, 4.6, 5.4, and 6.0 m/s) respectively. Ten trials for each ball speed, which were arranged with counter-balancing. Nine kinematic parameters and 3 velocity related to action-scaled conducted parameters were collected as dependent variables. One-way ANOVA with repeated measures on ball speed found that intercepting faster ball with one-hand brought about: (1) increasing the maximal hand aperture with more stable variability, (2) modifying movement faster so that the relative latency time and movement time became shorter, (3) regulating forward distance so that the displacement of transportation and grasping phase became shorter, and (4) the action-scaled ratios was higher and it was changed to stay at specific boundaries after on the certain of threshold.
彭國威、卓俊伶、楊梓楣 (2009)。伸手抓取動作之身體尺度比率界限。體育學報,42(3),63-76。new window

彭國威、卓俊伶、楊梓楣 (2010)。抓取之手指數與身體尺度比率的年齡差異。體育學報,43(4),9-24。new window

蕭雅文 (1997)。聽力學導論。臺北市:五南。

舊約 (1987)。詩篇。載於 Shangti(主編),舊新約全書。香港:香港聖經協會。

Abernethy, B., & Burgess-Limerick, R. (1992). Visual information for the timing of skilled movements: A review. In J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 343-384). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Abernethy, B., & Saprrow, W. A. (1992). The rise and fall of dominant paradigms in motor behaviour research. In J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 3-45). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Alderson, G. J. K., Sully, D. L., & Sully, H. G. (1974). An operational analysis of a one-handed catching task using high speed photography. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 217-226.

Beek, P. J., & Meijer, O. G. (1988). On the nature of “the” motor-action controversy. In O.G. Meijer & K. Roth (Eds.), Complex movement behavior: The motor-action controversy (pp. 157-185). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Berg, W. P., Wade, M. G., & Greer, N. L. (1993). Direction of gaze in real-world bipedal locomotion. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 24, 49-70.

Bockemühl, T., Troje, N. F., & Dürr, V. (2010). Inter-joint coupling and joint angle synergies of human catching movements. Human Movement Science, 29, 73-93.

Bootsma, R. J., Marteniuk, R. G., MacKenzie, C. L., & Zaal, F. T. J. M. (1994). The speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: Effects of movement amplitude, object size and object width on kinematic characteristics. Experimental Brain Research, 98, 535-541.

Burton, A. W., & Davis, W. E. (1992). Optimizing the involvement and performance of children with physical impairments in movement activities. Pediatric Exercise Science, 4, 236-248.

Burton, A. W., & Davis, W. E. (1996). Ecological task analysis: Utilizing intrinsic measures in research and practice. Human Movement Science, 15, 285-314.

Cesari, P., & Newell, K. M. (1999). The scaling of human grip configurations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 927-935.

Cesari, P., & Newell, K. M. (2000). Body scaling of grip configuration in children aged 6-12 years. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 301-310.

Chemero, A. (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 15, 181-195.

Cutting, J. E. (1986). Perception with an eye for motion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Davis, W. E., & Burton, A. W. (1991). Ecological task analysis: Translating movement behavior theory into practice. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 8, 154-177.

Davids, K., Savelsbergh, G., Bennett, S. J., & van der Kamp, J. (2002). Interceptive actions in sport: Information and movement. New York: Routledge.

Dicks, M., Davids, K., & Button, C. (2010). Individual differences in the visual control of intercepting a penalty kick in association football. Human Movement Science, 29, 401-411.

Elliott, J. M., & Connolly, K. J. (1984). A classification of manipulative hand movements. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 26, 283-296.

Fajen, B. R. (2005). Perceiving possibilities for action: On the necessity of calibration and perceptual learning for the visual guidance of action. Perception, 34, 717-740.

Fitch, H. L., & Turvey, M. T. (1978). On the control of activity: Some remarks from an ecological point of view. In D. M. Landers & R. W. Christina (Eds.), Psychology of motor behavior and sport – 1977 (pp. 3-35). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Gallahue, D. L., & Ozmun, J. C. (2002). Understanding motor development: Infants, children, adolescents, adults (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Gardner, D. L., Mark, L. S., Ward, J. A., & Edkins, H. (2001). How do task characteristics affect the transitions between seated and standing reaches? Ecological Psychology, 13, 245-274.

Gentile, A. M. (2000). Skill acquisition: Action, movement, and neuromotor processes. In J. H. Carr & R. B. Shepherd (Eds.), Movement science: Foundations for physical therapy in rehabilitation (2nd ed.) (pp. 111-187). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.

Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. E. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 67-82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Gibson, J. J. (1979/1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Greeno, J. G. (1994). Gibson’s affordances. Psychological Review, 101, 336-342.

Guthrie, E. R. (1935/1960). The psychology of learning. New York: Harper & Brothers.

Jeannerod, M. (1981). Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 153-168). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Jeannerod, M. (1984). The timing of natural prehension movement. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 235-254.

Jeannerod, M. (1992). Coordination mechanisms in prehension movement. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior II (pp. 265-285). New York: Elsevier Science.

Jeannerod, M., & Marteniuk, R. G. (1992). Functional characteristics of prehension: From data to artificial neural networks. In L. Proteau & D. Elliott (Eds.), Vision and motor control (pp. 197-232). New York: Elsevier Science.

Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 3-47). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). On the control and co-ordination of naturally developing systems. In J. A. S. Kelso & J. E. Clark (Eds.), The development of movement control and co-ordination (pp. 5-78). New York: Wiley.

Kuhn, T. S. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions (3rd ed.). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Laurent, M., Montagne, G., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (1994). The control and coordination of one-handed catching: The effect of temporal constraints. Experimental Brain Research, 101, 314-322.

Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5, 437-459.

Lee, D. N. (1980). The optic flow field: The foundation of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 290(B), 169-179.

Lee, D. N., & Reddish, P. E. (1981). Plummeting gannets: A paradigm of ecological optics. Nature, 293, 293-294.

MacKenzie,C. L., Marteniuk, R. G., Dugas, C., Liske, D., & Eickmeier, B. (1987). Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts’ task: Implications for control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39(A), 629-647.

Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 361-370.

Marteniuk, R. G., Leavitt, J. L., MacKenzie, C. L., & Athenes, S. (1990). Functional relationships between grasp and transport components in a prehension task. Human Movement Science, 9, 149-176.
Mazyn, L. I. N., Montagne, G., Savelsbergh, G. J. P., & Lenoir, M. (2006). Reorganization of catching coordination under varying temporal constraints. Motor Control, 10, 143-159.

Mazyn, L. I. N., Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Montagne, G., & Lenoir, M. (2007). Planning and on-line control of catching as a function of perceptual-motor constraints. Acta Psychologica, 126, 59-78.

McLeod, P., McLaughlin, C., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1985). Information encapsulation and automaticity: Evidence from the visual control of finely timed actions. In M. I. Posner & O. S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 391-406). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Michaels, C. F., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Michaels, C. F., & Oudejans, R. R. D. (1992). The optics of catching fly balls: Zeroing out optical acceleration. Ecological Psychology, 4, 199-222.

Montagne, G., Laurent, M., & Ripoll, H. (1993). Visual information pick-up in ball-catching. Human Movement Science, 12, 273-297.

Napier, J. R. (1956). The prehensile movements of the human hand. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery, 38B, 902-913.

Newell, K. M. (1985). Coordination, control and skill. In D. Goodman, R. B. Wilberg, & I. M. Franks (Eds.), Differing perspectives in motor learning, memory, and control (pp. 295-317). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In M. G. Wade & H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341-360). Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.

Newell, K. M., Scully, D. M., Tenenbaum, F., & Hardiman, S. (1989). Body scale and the development of prehension. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 1-14.

Newell, K. M., & Slifkin, A. B. (1998). The nature of movement variability. In J. P. Piek (Ed.), Motor behavior and human skill (pp. 143-160). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Oldfiled, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-112.

Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F., & Davids, K. (1999). Shedding some light on catching in the dark: Perceptual mechanisms for catching fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 531-542.

Peper, L., Bootsma, R. J., Mestre, D. R., & Bakker, F. (1994). Catching balls: How to get the hand to the right place at the right time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 591-612.

Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Whiting, H. T. A., & Bootsma, R. J. (1991). Grasping tau. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 315-322.

Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Whiting, H. T. A., & Pijpers, J. R. (1992). The control of catching. In J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and Learning (pp. 313-342). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Whiting, H. T. A., Pijpers, J. R., & van Santvoord, A. M. M. (1993). The visual guidance of catching. Experimental Brain Research, 93, 146-156.

Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225-260.

Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2011). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Sharp, R. H., & Whiting, H. T. A. (1974). Exposure and occluded duration effects in a ball-catching skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 139-147.

Snyder, R. G., Schneider, L. W., Owing, C. L., Reynolds, H. M., Golomb, D. H., & Schork, M. A. (1977). Anthropometry in infants, children and youths to age 18 for product safety designs. Warrendale, PA: Society for Automotive Engineers.

Stoffregen, T. A. (2000). Affordances and events. Ecological Psychology, 12, 1-28.

Stoffregen, T. A. (2003). Affordances are enough: Reply to Chemero et al. (2003). Ecological Psychology, 15, 29-36.

Stoffregen, T. A. (2004). Breadth and limits on the affordance concept. Ecological Psychology, 16, 79-85.

Turvey, M. T. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist, 45, 938-953.

Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (1986). The ecological approach to perceiving-acting: A pictorial essay. Acta Psychologica, 63, 133-155.

Turvey, M. T., Fitch, H. L., & Tuller, B. (1982). The Bernstein perspective: I. The problems of degrees of freedom and context-conditioned variability. In J. A. S. Kelso (Ed.), Human motor behavior: An introduction (pp. 239-252). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair-climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683-703.

Warren, W. H. (1995). Constructing an econiche. In J. Flach, P. Hancock, J. Caird, & K. Vicente (Eds.), Global perspectives on the ecology of human-machine systems (pp. 210-237). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Warren, W. H., & Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through apertures: Body-scaled information for affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 371-383.

Webster's Online Dictionary with multilingual thesaurus translation (2001). adaptation. Retrieved from http://www.websters-online-dictionary.com/definitions/adaptation?cx=partner-pub-0939450753529744%3Av0qd01-tdlq&cof=FORID%3A9&ie=UTF-8&q=adaptation&sa=Search#922

Webster’s revised unabridged dictionary (1998). intercept. Retrieved from http://www.websters-online-dictionary.org/definitions/intercept?cx=partner-pub-0939450753529744%3Av0qd01-tdlq&cof=FORID%3A9&ie=UTF-8&q=intercept&sa=Search#922.

Wing, A. M., Turton, A., & Fraser, C. (1986). Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 245-260.

Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual perception and action in sport. New York: E & FN SPON.
Zaal, F. T. J. M., & Bootsma, R. J. (1993). Accuracy demands in natural prehension. Human Movement Science, 12, 339-345.
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
QR Code
QRCODE