:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:逆事實思考預防功能之研究:由記憶狀態與心靈污染觀點探討之
作者:黃玉凰 引用關係
作者(外文):Yu-Huang Huang
校院名稱:元智大學
系所名稱:管理研究所
指導教授:廖淑伶
沈永正
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2007
主題關鍵詞:逆事實思考心靈污染記憶狀態預防意圖再購意願counterfactual thinkingmental contaminationmemory statespreventing intentionrepurchase intention
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(1) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:1
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:53
許多時候,消費者在決策過程中,會去想像另一種不同於過去已發生結果的情況,這時消費者便涉及所謂「逆事實思考」的消費者行為。茲以「如果我當時買的是福特而非克萊斯勒的話,我就不用花那麼多錢」為例,逆事實思考的過程通常是以諸如「要是當初我不這麼做;而是那麼做,會怎樣」的條件陳述語句作為呈現,因此包括某一前因與後果。而自Roese(1997)將『逆事實』的觀念帶入決策過程中,並將之定義為過去已發生結果的替代版本後,相關研究便陸續出現。
其中,許多關於逆事實思考的研究皆指出向上逆事實思考可作為消費者未來制定決策時的參考借鏡,而由現實生活中我們亦發現此議題與我們生活的密切性及其在消費者行為研究上的重要性。然而,相關實證性研究資料卻極盡缺乏。故本研究首先探究由向上逆事實思考觸發、消費者再購時的預防機轉效果。另外,本研究亦發現對心靈認知缺陷的理解是洞悉消費者心理與行為的關鍵之一,因此,我們引用Wilson and Brekke(1994)提出之心靈污染(Mental Contamination)的概念作為本研究思維辨證與推論的依據。
本研究共發展出三個研究模型。模型一中,本研究先探討向上逆事實思考預防功能之效果以及記憶狀態與情境狀況對於向上逆事實思考所引發的預防意圖效果之影響。接著,鑒於不同記憶狀態下,所引發或產生的心靈污染變數不盡相同,故本研究再依記憶狀態的不同,先在模型二中探究記憶清晰狀態下,所產生的心靈污染變數及其對預防意圖效果的影響;最後再於模型三中討論記憶模糊狀態下所產生的心靈污染變數及其對預防意圖效果的影響。
本研究藉由虛擬故事法設計問卷,分別對不同記憶狀態下可能的心靈污染變數進行具體化地操弄,再運用SPSS與SAS統計軟體分析,以實證方式進行研究驗證並得到如後的結論:(1)向上逆事實思考所引發之預防意圖的確會影響消費者對品牌選擇的機率;(2)記憶狀態對預防意圖在舊品牌再購機率上的作用的確會產生干擾效果;(3)資訊類型對預防意圖在品牌選擇機率作用的干擾效果不顯著;(4)在記憶狀態清晰下,各心靈污染變數對預防意圖在舊品牌再購機率上的作用的確會產生干擾效果;(5)在記憶狀態模糊下,各心靈污染變數對預防意圖在品牌選擇機率作用的干擾效果不顯著;(6)當記憶清晰時,心靈污染變數將更容易對預防意圖作用產生干擾效果;(7)舊品牌與新品牌間的選擇機率為獨立非完全相關。最後,本研究更據此討論管理義涵並發展行銷實務之建議。
Oftentimes, consumers imagine the particular situation which is very opposite of what had happened before. The consumer is, at this moment, involved in the process of so-called “counterfactual thinking”. People usually use the expression such as “if I had not done this, I would have…” to demonstrate the process of counterfactual thinking. For example, the individual may use the sentence, such as “If I had not bought Chrysler but Ford instead, I would not have spent so much money”, to describe the causes and effects of his or her regrets. In 1997, Roese (1997) started to apply this concept to the field of decision-making. From then on, related studies have been coming out successively.
Some researches about the concept of counterfactual thinking have pointed out that the upward counterfactual thinking could lead consumers to a better decision. Although the concept is closely related to our daily life and its importance is also obvious in the field of consumer behavior, related studies are short on empirical orientation. Therefore, this research explores, in the beginning, consumers’ preventing effects caused by upward counterfactual thinking while they are considering buying the similar product again. This research supports the idea that the understanding of mental cognition is one of the keys to detect consumers’ behaviors, so we use the concept of Mental Contamination coined by Wilson and Brekke (1994) as a theoretical framework in this research.
Three research models have been developed in this research. In model one, the prevent effects of upward counterfactual thinking and the interference effects of memory and situation states have been discussed. Because different memory states may result in different variables in terms of mental contamination, the researcher explores, in model two, consumers’ mental contamination variables when they are in the state of clear memory. The influences of the variables on the prevent effects are also demonstrated in this model. The researcher finally discusses the influences of mental contamination variables on the prevent effects when consumers’ memory is not clear.
The researcher develops questionnaires with scenario and manipulates the mental contamination variables under different memory states separately. The statistic softwares such as SPSS and SAS have been employed to analyze the empirical data. The conclusions of this research are as follows: 1. the effects of preventing intention derived from upward counterfactual thinking will influence the probability of consumers’ choice of brand. 2. consumers’ memory states will interfere in the preventing intention referring to the probability of the choice of the old brand. 3. the interference effects of information types on the preventing intention referring to the probability of brand choice are not significant. 4. each mental contamination variable will interfere in the preventing intention referring to the probability of the choice of the old brand when the consumer is in the state of clear memory. 5. the interference effects of each mental contamination variable on the preventing intention referring to the probability of brand choice are not significant when the consumer’s memory is not clear. 6. the mental contamination variable will easily interfere in the preventing intention when the consumer’s memory is clear. 7. the probabilities between the choice of the old brand and that of the new brand are independent and have imperfect correlation. Based on the conclusions such as the above, the researcher discusses, in the final part of this research, related managerial insights and gives suggestions on marketing practice.
1.Abraham, M. M., & Lodish, L. M. (1990). Getting the most out of advertising and promotion. Harvard Business Review, 68, 50-58.
2.Allport, G. W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
3.Anderson, J. R. (1983). The Architecture of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
4.Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In Uleman, J. S. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Unintended Thought (pp. 3-51). New York: Guilford Press.
5.Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of Motivation and Cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 93-130). New York: Guilford Press.
6.Bargh, J. A. (1992). Does subliminality matter to social psychology? Awareness of the stimulus versus awareness of its influence. In Bornstein, R. & Pittman, T. (Eds.), Perception without awareness (pp. 236-255). New York: Guilford.
7.Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automaticity in social psychology. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 169-183). New York: Guilford Press.
8.Berkman, H. W., & Gilson, C. (1986). Consumer Behavior: Concept and Strategies.(3rd ed.). Boston, Mass.: Kent Pub.
9.Berkowitz, L., & Troccoli, B. T. (1990). Feeling, direction of attention, and expressed evaluations of others. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 305-325.
10.Bettman, J. R., & Park, C. W. (1980). Effects of prior knowledge and experience and phase of the choice process on consumer decision processes: a protocol analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 234-248.
11.Billig, M. (1985). Prejudice, categorization, and particularization: From a perceptual to a rhetorical approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 79-103.
12.Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping responses and social resources in attenuating the stress of life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139–57.
13.Boninger, D. S., Gleicher, F., & Strathman, A. (1994). Counterfactual thinking: From what might have been to what may be. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 297-307.
14.Brainerd, C. J., Stein, L. M., & Reyna, V. F. (1998). On the development of conscious and unconscious memory. Developmental Psychology, 34, 342-357.
15.Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1971). The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 331-350.
16.Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R., & Franks, J. J. (1972). Sentence memory: A constructive versus interpretive approach. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 193-209.
17.Braun, K. A. (1999). Postexperience advertising effects on consumer memory. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(4), 319-334.
18.Brendl, C. M., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Principles of judging valence: What masks events positive or negative? In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (28, pp. 95-160). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
19.Brewer, M. B. (1989). A dual process model of impression formation. In Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. K. (Eds.), Advances in Social Cognition(1, pp. 1-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
20.Brown, J. D. (1990). Evaluating one''s abilities: Shortcuts and stumbling blocks on the road to self-knowledge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 149-167.
21.Buck, M. L., & Miller, D. T. (1994). Reactions to incongruous negative life events. Social Justice Research, 7, 29-46.
22.Burke, R. R., & Srull, T. K. (1988). Competitive interference and consumer memory for advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(June), 55-68.
23.Buschke, H. (1973). Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 543-550.
24.Colin, C., Babcock, L., Loewenstein, G., & Thaler, R. (1997). Labor supply of New York City cabdrivers: One day at a time. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 407-442.
25.Chintagunta, P. K., & Honore, B. E. (1996). Investigating the effects of marketing variables and unobserved heterogeneity in a multinomial probit model. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 13(1), 1-15.
26.Collins, R. L. (1996). For better or worse: The impact of upward social comparison on self-evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 51-69.
27.Cummings, E. M., Davies P. T., & Simpson, K. S. (1994). Marital conflict gender and children’s appraisals and coping efficacy as mediators of child adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 8(2), 141–49.
28.Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., & Thompson, S. C. (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 109-124.
29.Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate Recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17-22.
30.Devine, P. G. (1989a). Automatic and controlled processes in prejudice: The role of stereotypes and personal beliefs. In Pratkanis, A. R., Breckler, S. J. & Greenwald, A. G. (Eds.), Attitude Structure and Function, (pp. 181-212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
31.Devine, P. G. (1989b). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5-18.
32.Dolinski, D., Gromski, W., & Zawisza, E. (1987). Unrealistic pessimism. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 511-516.
33.Drake, R. A. (1987). Conceptions of own versus others’ outcomes: Manipulation by monaural attentional orientation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 373-375.
34.Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg , A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definition in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1082-1090.
35.Dutton, D. G., & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 510-517.
36.Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1973). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381-391.
37.Engel, J. F., Blackwell, R. D., & Miniard, P. W. (1995). Consumer Behavior. New York: The Dryden Press.
38.Engel, J. F., Blackwell, R. D., & Miniard, P. W. (1993). Consumer Behavior (7th ed.). New York: The Dryden Press.
39.Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G. (1988). Self-generated validity and other effects of measurement on beliefs, attitude, intention, and behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73 (August), 421-435.
40.Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, III: Row and Peterson.
41.Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203 – 210.
42.Fillenbaum, S. (1974). Information amplified: Memory for counterfactual conditionals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 44-49.
43.Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample. Journal of Health Social Behavior, 21, 219–39.
44.Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 53-60.
45.Freud, A. (1946). The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defense. New York: International University Press.
46.Gabrielcik, A., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). Priming and frequency estimation: A strict test of the availability heuristic. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 85-89.new window
47.Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 48, 107-119.
48.Gilbert, D. T. (1993). The assent of man: Mental representation and the control of belief. In Wegner, D. M., & Pennebaker, J. W. (Eds.), The Handbook of Mental Control( pp. 57-87). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
49.Gilbert, D. T., & Osborne, R. E. (1989). Thinking backward: Some curable and incurable consequences of cognitive busyness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 940-949.
50.Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, when and why. Psychological Review, 102, 379-395.
51.Girotto, V., Legrenzi, P., & Rizzo, A. (1991). Event controllability in counterfactual thinking. Acta Psychologia, 78, 111-133.
52.Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., Strathman, A., Armor, D., & Ahn, M. (1995). With an eye toward the future: The impact of counterfactual thinking on affect, attitudes, and behaviour. In Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (Eds.), What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking (pp. 283-304). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
53.Gleicher, F., Kost, K. A., Baker, S. M., Strathman, A. J., Richman, S. A., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). The role of counterfactual thinking in judgments of affects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16 (2), 284-295.
54.Golding, J. M., Fowler, S. B., Long, D. L., & Latta, H. (1990). Instructions to disregard potentially useful information: The effects of pragmatics on evaluative judgments and recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 212-227.
55.Goodman, N. (1947). The problem of counterfactual conditionals. Journal of Philosophy, 44, 113-128.
56.Gupta, S., Chintagunta, P., Kaul, A., & Wittink, D. R. (1996). Do household scanner data provide representative inferences from brand choices: A comparison with store data? Journal of Marketing Research, 33, 383-398.
57.Haan N., (1969). A tripartite model of ego functioning: Values and clinical research applications. Journal of Nerve Meningitis, 148, 14-30.
58.Harkins, D. I., Best, R. J., and Coney, K. A. (1991). Consumer Behavior: Implications for Marketing Strategy. IRWIN, (5th ed.).
59.Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory: An Experimental and Systematic Approach to Behavior. New York: Harper & Row.
60.Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1992). Unconscious sources of subjectivity and suffering: Is consciousness the solution. In Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The Construction of Social Judgments (pp. 67-103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
61.Higgins, E. T. (1989). Knowledge accessibility and activation: Subjectivity and suffering from unconscious sources. In Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Unintended Thought (pp. 75-123). New York: Guilford Press.
62.Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessiblity, applcabolity, and salience. In Higgins, E. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (pp. 133-168). New York: Guilford Press.
63.Higgins, E. T., & King, G. (1981). Accessibility of social constructs: Information-processing consequences of individual and contextual variability. In Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (Eds.), Personality, Cognition, and Social Interaction (pp. 69-121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
64.Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. W., & Jones, C. R. (1997). Category accessibility and impression formation. In Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (Eds.), Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (13, pp. 141-154).
65.Hilton, D. J. & Slugoski, B. R. (1986). Knowledge-based causal attribution: The abnormal conditions focus model. Psychological Review, 93, 65-88.
66.Horowitz, M. (1976). Stress Response Syndromes. New York: Jason Aronson.
67.Hsee, C., Yu, F., Zhang, J. J., & Zhang, Y. (2003). Medium macimization. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 1-14.
68.Hugstad, P., Taylor, J. W., & Bruce, G. D. (1980). The effects of social class and perceived risk on consumer information search. Journal of Services Marketing, 1 (1), 47-53.
69.Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306-340.
70.Jacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1987). Unconscious influence of memory for a prior event. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 1314-336.
71.Jacoby, L. L., & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 126-135.
72.Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., & Dywan, J. (1989). Memory attribution. In Craik, F. I. M. (Ed.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp.391-422). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
73.Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, S. D., & Toth, J. P. (1992). Unconscious influences revealed: Attention, awareness, and control. American Psychologist, 47, 802-809.
74.Jacoby, L. L., Woloshyn, V., & Kelley, C. M. (1989). Becoming famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 115-125.
75.Janis, I., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision Making. New York: Free Press.
76.Johnson, J. T. (1986). The knowledge of what might have been: affective and attribution consequences of near outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(1), 51-62.
77.Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological Review, 88, 67-85.
78.Johnson, M. K., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). Constructing and reconstructing the past and the future in the present. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Hand Book of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior (2, pp. 482-526). New York: Guilford Press.
79.Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, H., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3-28.
80.Kahneman, D. & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychology Review, 93(2), 136-153.
81.Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237-251.
82.Kahneman, D., & Varey, C. A. (1990). Propensities and counterfactuals: The loser that almost won. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1101-1110.
83.Kahneman, D. (1995). Varieties of counterfactual thinking. In Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (Eds.), What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
84.Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In Kahneman, D. et al. (ed.), Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases (pp. 201-208). New York: Cambridge.
85.Kahneman, D., & Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts: A cognitive perspective on risk taking. Management Science, 39(1), 17-31.
86.Kahneman, D., & Riep, M. W. (1998). Aspects of investor psychology. Journal of Portfolio Management, 24(4), 52-65.
87.Keller, P. A., Lipkus, I. M., & Rimer, B. K. (2002). Depressive realism and health risk accuracy: The negative consequences of positive mood. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 57-69.
88.Kelly, H. H. (1972). Attribution in social interaction. In Jones, E. E., Kanouse, D. E., Kelly, R. E., Nisbett, S. V., & Weiner, B. (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the Cause of Behavior (pp. 1-26). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
89.Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445-1452.
90.Kirscht, J. P., Haefner, D. P., Kegekes, S. S., & Rosenstock (1966). A national study of health beliefs. Journal of Health and Human Behavior, 7, 248-254.
91.Klauer, K. C., & Migulla, G. (1995). Spontaneous counterfactual processing. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 26, 34-42.
92.Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Memory metaphors and the real-life/laboratory controversy: correspondence versus storehouse conceptions of memory. Behavioral Brain Science, 19, 167-228.
93.Koriat, A., Goldsmith, M., & Pansky, A. (2000). Toward a psychology of memory accuracy. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 481-537.
94.Kotler, P. (1986). Principles of Marketing. (3rd). New Jersey: Englewood Cliffs.
95.Kotler, P. (1997). Marketing Management: Analysis Planning, Implementation and Control. (8th Ed). New Jersey:Prentice Hall Inc.
96.Kuiper, N. A., & MacDonald, M. R. (1983). Self and other perception in mild depressives. Social Cognition, 1, 223-239.
97.Kuiper, N. A., & Derry, P. A. (1982). Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 286-297.
98.Landman, J. (1993). Regret: The Persistence of the Possible. New York: Oxford University Press.
99.Landman, J. (1987). Regret: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Journal of Theory of Social Behavior, 17 (June), 135-160.
100.Landman, J. and Petty, R. (2000). It could have been you: How states exploit counterfactual thought to market lotteries. Psychology & Marketing, 17 (4), 299-321.
101.Landy, D., & Sigall, H. (1974). Beauty is talent: Task evaluation as a function of the performer’s physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 299-304.
102.Lanman, J. (1987). Regret and elation following action and inaction: Affective responses to positive versus negative outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 524-536.
103.Larwood, L. (1978). Swine flu: A field study of self-serving bias. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 231-250.
104.LauxL, W. H. (1991). Presentation of self in coping with anger and anxiety: an intentional approach. Anxiety Research, 3(4), 233–55.
105.Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal and Coping. New York: Springer.
106.Lee, C. (1989). Perceptions of immunity to disease in an adult smoker. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 267-277.
107.Lehman, D. R., Lempert, R. O., & Nisbett, R. E. (1988). The effects of graduate training on reasoning. American Psychologist, 43, 431-442.
108.Lehmann, D. R., Moore, W. L., & Elrod, T. (1982). The development of distinct choice process segments over time: a stochastic modeling approach. Journal of Marketing, 46(2), 48-60.
109.Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., & Nerenz, D. (1980). The commonsense representation of illness danger. Contributions to Medical Psychology, 2, 7-30.
110.Levitt, T. (1960). Marketing myopia. Harvard Business Review, 38, 24-47.
111.Lewicki, P. (1986). No Conscious Social Information Pressing. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
112.Loftus, E. F. (1997). Creating false memories. Scientific American, 277, 50-55.
113.Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585-589.
114.Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19-31.
115.Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of autoimmunization. Psychological Review, 95, 492-527.
116.Logan, G. D. (1989). Automatic and cognitive control. In Uleman, J. S. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Unintended Thought (pp. 52-74). New York: Guilford Press.
117.Lombardi, W. J., Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). The role of consciousness in priming effects on categorization: Assimilation versus contrast as a function of awareness of the priming task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 411-429.
118.Luce, M. F. & Kahn, B. E. (1999). Avoidance or vigilance? The psychology of false-positive test results. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 242-259.new window
119.Lund, F. H. (1925). The psychology of belief. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 20, 63-81.
120.Lynch, J. G., & Srull, T. K. (1982). Memory and attentional factors in consumer choice: Concepts and research methods. Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (June), 18-37.
121.Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. (1993). The mental simulation of better and worse possible worlds. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 87-109.
122.Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., McMullen, M. N. (1995). The impact of perceived control on the imagination of better and worse possible worlds. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 (6), 588-595.
123.Marshall, G. D., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1979). Affective consequences of inadequately explained physiological arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 970-988.
124.Martin, L. L. (1986). Set/reset: Use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 493-504.
125.Martin, L. L., Seta, J. J., & Crelia, R. A. (1990). Assimilation and contrast as a function of people’s willingness and ability to expend effort in forming an impression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 27-37.
126.Maslach, C. (1979). Negative emotional biasing of unexplained arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 953-969.
127.McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality coping and coping effectiveness in an adult sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 385–405.
128.McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (1998). Attempting to avoid illusory memories: Robust false recognition of associates persists under conditions of explicit warnings and immediate testing. Journal of Memory & Language, 39, 508-520.
129.Medin, D. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44, 1469-1481.
130.Medvec, V. H., Madey, S. F., & Gilovich, T. (1995). When less is more: Counterfactual thinking and satisfaction among Olympic medalists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 603-610.
131.Menninger, K. (1963). The Vital Balance: The Life Process in Mental Health and Illness. New York: Viking.
132.Menon, G., Block, L. G., & Ramanathan, S. (2002). We’re at as much risk as we’re led to believe: Effects of message cues on judgments of health risk. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 533-549.
133.Meyers-Levy, J., & Maheswaran, D. (1992). When timing matters: The influence of temporal distance on consumer’s affective and persuasive responses. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 424-433.
134.Miller, D. T., & Taylor, B. R. (1995). Counterfactual thought, regret, and superstition: How to avoid kicking yourself. In Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (Eds.), What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking (pp. 305-331).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
135.Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1989). Counterfactual thinking and social perception: Thinking about what might have been. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 305-331). New York: Academic Press.
136.Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1989). When a coincidence is suspicious: The role of mental simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 581-589.
137.Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & Mcfarland, C. (1990). Counterfactual thinking and social perception: Thinking about what might have been. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (23, pp.305-331). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
138.Miller, D. T., Taylor, B., & Buck, M. L. (1991). Gender gaps: Who needs to be explained? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 5.
139.Huang, M. H. (1999). Are consumers inherently or situational brand loyal?--A set interco relation account for conscious brand loyalty and no conscious inertia. Psychology & Marketing, 16 (6), 523-545
140.N’gbala, A., & Branscombe, N. R. (1995). Mental simulation and causal attribution: When simulating an event does not affect fault assignment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 139-162.
141.Newman, J. W., & Staelin, R. (1972). Repurchase information seeking for new cars and major household appliances. Journal of Marketing Research, 9, August, 249-257.
142.Niedenthal, P., Tangney, J., and Gavanski, I. (1994). “If only I weren’t” versus “If only I hadn’t”: Distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 585–595.
143.Nisbett, R. E., & Schachter, S. (1966). Cognitive manipulation of pain. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2, 227-236.
144.Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231-259.
145.Olson, J. M. (1990). Self-inference processes in emotion. In Olson, J. M., & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.), Self- inference Processes: The Ontario Symposium (6, pp. 17-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
146.Olson, J. M., Rose, N. J., & Zanna, M. P. (1996). Expectancies. In Higgins, E. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (pp. 211-238). New York: Guilford Press.
147.Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 33-60.
148.Perloff, L. S., & Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self-other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 502-510.
149.Petty, R. E., Schumann, D., Richman, S., & Strathman, A. (1993). Positive mood and persuasion: Different roles for affect under high and low elaboration conditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5-20.
150.Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Intentional chapters on unintended thoughts. In Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Unintended Thought (pp. 450-469). New York: Guilford Press.
151.Raghubir, P., & Menon, G. (1998). AIDS and me, never the twain shall meet: The effects of information accessibility on judgments of risk and advertising effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 52-63.
152.Reisenzein, R. (1983). The Schachter theory of mention: Two decades later. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 239-264.
153.Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning & Individual Differences, 7, 1-75.
154.Reyna, V. F., & Lloyd, F. (1997). Theories of false memory in children and adults. Learning & Individual Differences, 9, 95-123.
155.Robertson, L. S. (1977). Car crashes: Perceived vulnerability and willingness to pay for crash protection. Journal of Community Health, 7, 248-254.
156.Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803-814.
157.Roese, N. J., & Olson, J.M. (1997). Counterfactual thinking: The intersection of affect and function. In Zanna, M. P. (Eds.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 1-59). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
158.Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1993). Self-esteem and counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 199-206.
159.Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121( 1), 133-148.
160.Rosch, E. (1983). Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E., & Lloyd, B. (Eds.), Cognition and Categorization (pp. 27-48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
161.Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Outcome controllability and counterfactual thinking. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 620-628.
162.Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805-818.
163.Roy, R., Chintagunta, P. K., & Haldar, S. (1996). A framework for investigating habits, the hand of the past, and heterogeneity in dynamic brand choice. Marketing Science, 15 (3) 280-299.
164.Ryan, B. J. (1991). It works! How investment spending in advertising pays off. New York: American Association of Advertising Agencies.
165.Sanna, L. J., & Turley, K. J. (1996). Antecedents to spontaneous counterfactual thinking: Effects of expectancy violation and outcome valence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 906-920.
166.Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and psychological determinants of emotion. Psychological Review, 69, 379-399.
167.Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., & Pradere, D. (1996). The neuropsychology of memory illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesic patients. Journal of Memory & Language, 35, 319-334.
168.Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, E. T., & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Hand Book of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior (2, pp. 527-561). New York: Guilford Press.
169.Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its alternatives: An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The Construction of Social Judgments (pp. 217-245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
170.Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513-523.
171.Sedklides, C. (1992). Mood as a determinant of intentional focus. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 129-148.
172.Sherman, S., & McConnell, A. (1995). Dysfunctional implications of counterfactual thinking: When alternatives to reality fail us. In Olson, J. (Ed.), What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking? (pp. 199-232) Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
173.Shugan, Steve (1980). The cost of thinking. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 99-111.
174.Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 29–39.
175.Shim, S., Gehrt, K., & Lotz, S. (2002). Export implications for the Japanese fruit market: Fruit-specific lifestyle segments. International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 29(6), 298-314.
176.Simth, E., & Medin, D. (1981). Categories and Concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
177.Solomon, M. R. (1994). Consumer Behavior: Buying, Having and Being. (2nd ed.). Allyn and Bacon.
178.Sood, S., Rottenstreich, Y., & Brenner, L. (2004). On decisions that lead to decisions: Direct and derived evaluations of preference. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), 17-25.
179.Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1980). Category accessibility and social perception: Some implications for the study of person memory and interpersonal judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 841-856.
180.Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Schwarz, N. (1988). Priming and communication: Social determinants of information use in judgments of life satisfaction. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 429-442.
181.Strack, F., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Kübler, A., & Wänke, M. (1993). Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation versus contrast. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 53-62.
182.Sue, S., Smith, R. E., & Caldwell, C. (1973). Effects of inadmissible evidence on the decisions of simulated jurors: A moral dilemma. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 3, 345-353.
183.Swann, W. B. (1990). To be adored or to be known? The interplay of self-enhancement and self-verification. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior (2, pp. 408-448). New York: Guilford Press.
184.Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85.
185.Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210.
186.Taylor, S. E., & Schneider, S. K. (1989). Coping and the simulation of events. Social Cognition, 7, 174-194.
187.Taylor, S. E., Buunk, B. P., & Aspinwall, L. G. (1990). Social comparison, stress, and coping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 74.
188.Tetlock, P.E., & Belkin, A. (1996). Counterfactual Thought Experiments In World Politics: Logical, Methodological And Psychological Perspectives. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
189.Semon, T. T. (2001). Inertia a factor not to be ignored in research. Marketing News, June 18.
190.Thompson, W. C., Fong, G. T., & Rosenhan, D. L. (1981). Inadmissible evidence and juror verdicts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 453-463.
191.Wilson, T. D., Houston, C. E., & Meyers, J. M. (1998). Choose your poison: Effects of lay beliefs about mental processes on attitude change. Social Cognition, 16(1), 114-33.
192.Tulving, & Psotka, J. (1971). Retroactive inhibition in free recall: Inaccessibility of information available in the memory store. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 87, 1-8.
193.Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.
194.Udell, J. G. (1966). Prepurchase Behavior of buyers of small electrical appliance. Journal of Marketing, 30, 50-52.
195.Uleman, J. S. (1989). A framework for thinking intentionally about unintended thought. In Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The Construction of Social Judgments (pp. 425-449). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
196.Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adoption to Life. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.
197.Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Flexible correction processes in social judgment: The role of naïve theories in corrections for perceived bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 36-51.
198.Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1997). The flexible correction model: The role of naïve theories of bias in a bias correction. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (29, pp. 137-165). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
199.Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E., & Klein, D. J. (1994). Effects of mood on high elaboration attitude change: The mediating role of likelihood judgments. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 25-44.
200.Wegner, D. M. (1989). White Bears and other Unwanted Thoughts, New York: Viking Press.
201.Wegner, D. M. (1992). You can’t always think what you want: Problems in the suppression of unwanted thoughts. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (25, pp. 193-225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
202.Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52.
203.Wegner, D. M., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). The Handbook of Mental Control, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
204.Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806-820.
205.Weinstein, N. D. (1982). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 5, 441-460.
206.Weinstein, N. D., & Lachendro, E. (1982). Egocentrism as a source of unrealistic optimism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 195-200.
207.Wells, G.L., Taylor, B.R., & Turtle, J.W. (1987). The undoing of scenarios. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 421-430.
208.Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychology Bulletin, 116, 117-142.
209.Wilson, T. D., & Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The Construction of Social Judgments (pp. 37-65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
210.Wilson, T. D., Dunn, D. S., Kraft, D., & Lisle, D. J. (1989). Introspection, attitude change, and attitude-behavior consistency: The disruptive effects of explaining why we feel the way we do. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (19, pp. 123-205) San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
211.Wilson, T. D., Houston, C., Etling, K., & Brekke, N. (1993). Origins and Limits of Anchoring Effects. Unpublished manuscript, University of Virginia, Charlottesville.
212.Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and reseach concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106. 231-248.
213.Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused attention? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 899-908.
214.Wyer, R. S., & Budesheim, T. L. (1987). Person memory and judgments: The impact of information that one is told to disregard. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 14-29.
215.Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Human Memory and Cognition in Its Social Context. Hilisdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
216.Zillmann, D. (1978). Attribution and misattribution of excitatory reactions. In Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W., & Kidd, R. F. (Eds.), New Directions in Attribution Research (2, pp. 335-368). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
:::
無相關博士論文
 
無相關書籍
 
無相關著作
 
無相關點閱
 
QR Code
QRCODE