:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:是誰在恍神?探討個體注意力覺察傾向、身體覺察能力與工作記憶廣度在不同作業情境下對心神漫遊傾向的影響
作者:朱玉正
作者(外文):Yu-Jeng Ju
校院名稱:國立臺灣大學
系所名稱:心理學研究所
指導教授:連韻文
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2021
主題關鍵詞:心神漫遊工作記憶廣度正念覺察執行控制注意力mind wanderingworking memory capacitymindfulnessawarenessexecutive controlattention
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(0) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:1
當一個人的注意力從手邊的工作偏離而沈浸在其他無關想法時,就處於心神漫遊狀態而可能妨害作業表現。為了釐清過去有關心神漫遊在理論上與證據上的不一致,本論文以三個參與者內設計的實驗檢驗個體覺察傾向在不同作業情境下如何影響不同工作記憶廣度者認知資源的使用--用於調控以減少心神漫遊或是繼續維持心神漫遊。實驗一(N = 157)以常用於測量正念特質(mindfulness trait)中覺察當下特質的止觀傾向量表(Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale)代表個體注意力覺察傾向。結果顯示透過思維探針擷取的心神漫遊與工作記憶廣度的關係會受到個體注意力覺察傾向的調節,而且調節的效果也因作業認知負荷量改變而有所不同,支持我所提出的「心神漫遊的覺察調節假設」。實驗二(N = 82)則發現工作記憶廣度與覺察傾向無法預測由刺激誘發的心神漫遊傾向,然而在作業程序與難度相當但沒有明顯外在誘發刺激的注意力維持作業中,工作記憶廣度及注意力覺察傾向皆能預測心神漫遊傾向。實驗三(N = 171)以更高難度的作業、兩種心神漫遊指標(探針擷取以及作業反應時間的變異值)以及不同面向的覺察指標(注意力與身體覺察)重覆驗證實驗一。結果顯示在高難度的作業情境中,注意力覺察高低可以預測探針攫取的心神漫遊傾向,以及會調節高工作記憶廣度者的反應時間變異值。此外,本體覺覺察能力愈好的個體,該變異值愈低,特別對工作記憶廣度低者的影響更為明顯。本論文的發現有助於釐清影響心神漫遊發生到調節的歷程與機制,並對過去工作記憶廣度與心神漫遊間的關係提供一個整合的解釋。同時也首次發現本體覺覺察能力跟心神漫遊有關,而且其影響獨立於由上而下的調控途徑,有助於未來進一步探討正念相關訓練在注意力成效方面的可能機制。
Mind wandering (MW), a common experience in which one’s attention turns from the ongoing task toward task-unrelated thought, can impair performance on the task at hand. To clarify the inconsistencies between existing hypotheses and findings in MW research, I hypothesized that awareness would affect MW. Specifically, I hypothesized that one’s tendency to be aware of the present moment would affect the use of one’s cognitive resources with regard to reducing or maintaining MW, and this would in turn moderate the relationship between working memory capacity (WMC) and MW tendency. Three experiments following a within-participant design were conducted under different tasks. In Experiment 1 (N = 157), the Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale, a commonly used assessment of mindfulness, was administered to measure the trait of attentional awareness (AA). Consistent with the hypothesis, the WMC–MW tendency relationship, measured using thought probes, was moderated by AA, the effects of which varied under different task load conditions. In Experiment 2 (N = 82), in the condition that the MW mostly induced by external stimuli could not be predicted by either WMC or AA. However, in the control condition, in which MW was not induced by external stimuli, WMC and AA predicted MW tendency. Experiment 3 (N = 171) had the same design as Experiment 1 but featured more demanding tasks and involved the use of two MW indexes (thought probes and the response time coefficient of variation [RTCV]) as well as different facets of awareness (AA and body awareness). In a highly demanding condition, AA predicted MW tendency and moderated the relationship between WMC and RTCV. Moreover, participants with better proprioception awareness, especially those with low WMC, had smaller RTCVs. This study tested an integrative hypothesis regarding the mechanism of MW regulation. The findings provide a reasonable explanation for the conflicting evidence on the relationship between WMC and MW. To the best of my knowledge, this is the first report of a relationship between proprioception awareness and MW, and the related mechanism of how proprioception awareness could help MW regulation was discussed. The findings provide implications for mindfulness research on how mindfulness helps enhance attentional ability.
張仁和、林以正與黃金蘭(2011)。〈中文版「止觀覺察注意量表」之信效度分析〉。《測驗學刊》,58,90–115.
李少揚(2017):《身心互動途徑初探:以「身心中軸覺察訓練」對身體感覺、工作記憶與注意力控制功能的提升效果為例》(未出版之碩士論文)。國立臺灣大學,台北。
李茂寧(2019):《感覺你的身體:短期入班動態靜觀課程對學童內感覺、執行 控制功能、情緒調節及運動協調之介入成效》(未出版之碩士論文)。國立臺灣大學,台北。
翁宛婷(2020):《探討短期動態身心覺察課程對國小教師內感覺覺察、心理韌性與壓力知覺的介入成效》 (未出版之碩士論文)。國立臺灣師範大學,台北。
連韻文、鄧善娟、任純慧與祁業榮(2019)。〈禮樂傳統最優意識經驗的進路: 雅樂舞身心功效初探〉。《應用心理研究》。70. https://doi.org/10.3966/156092512019060070005
陳玉秀(2011):身心量覺的迴路。台北:財團法人原住民音樂文教基金會。
Ager, A. L., Roy, J.-S. S., Roos, M., Belley, A. F., Cools, A., & Hébert, L. J. (2017). Shoulder proprioception: How is it measured and is it reliable? A systematic review. Journal of Hand Therapy, 30(2), 221–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jht.2017.05.003
Alispahic, S., & Hasanbegovic-Anic, E. (2017). Mindfulness: Age and gender differences on a Bosnian sample. Psychological Thought, 10(1), 155–166. https://doi.org/10.5964/psyct.v10i1.224
Anderson, T., & Farb, N. A. S. (2020). The Metronome Counting Task for measuring meta-awareness. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01418-z
Anticevic, A., Cole, M. W., Murray, J. D., Corlett, P. R., Wang, X.-J., & Krystal, J. H. (2012). The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.008
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1849–1858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007
Asonitou, K., Koutsouki, D., Kourtessis, T., & Charitou, S. (2012). Motor and cognitive performance differences between children with and without developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(4), 996–1005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.01.008
Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528012.001.0001
Baer, R. a, Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504
Baird, B., Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Schooler, J. W., Baird, B., Mrazek, M. D., … Jonathan, W. (2014). Domain-specific enhancement of metacognitive ability following meditation training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(5), 1972–1979. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036882
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Fishman, D. J. F. F., Mrazek, M. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Unnoticed intrusions: Dissociations of meta-consciousness in thought suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 1003–1012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.06.009
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1117–1122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446024
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1604–1611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.007
Barron, E., Riby, L. M., Greer, J., & Smallwood, J. (2011). Absorbed in thought: The effect of mind wandering on the processing of relevant and irrelevant events. Psychological Science, 22(5), 596–601. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611404083
Bastian, M., & Sackur, J. (2013). Mind wandering at the fingertips: Automatic parsing of subjective states based on response time variability. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(SEP), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00573
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. 67(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Bixler R., D’Mello S. (2015) Automatic Gaze-Based Detection of Mind Wandering with Metacognitive Awareness. In: Ricci F., Bontcheva K., Conlan O., Lawless S. (eds) User Modeling, Adaptation and Personalization. UMAP 2015. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 9146. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-20267-9_3
Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 83–113. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015044
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539–546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003
Brewer, J. a, & Garrison, K. a. (2014). The posterior cingulate cortex as a plausible mechanistic target of meditation: findings from neuroimaging. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1307, 19–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12246
Brosowsky, N. P., DeGutis, J., Esterman, M., Smilek, D., & Seli, P. (2020). Mind wandering, motivation, and task performance over time: Evidence that motivation insulates people from the negative effects of mind wandering. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000263
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011
Carciofo, R., Song, N., Du, F., Wang, M. M., & Zhang, K. (2017). Metacognitive beliefs mediate the relationship between mind wandering and negative affect. Personality and Individual Differences, 107, 78–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.11.033
Carriere, J. S. a, Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031438
Casner, S. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Thoughts in flight: Automation use and pilots’ task-related and task-unrelated thought. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 56(3), 433–442. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720813501550
Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 303–322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0
Chin, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Meta-Awareness. In W. P. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Consciousness (pp. 33–41). University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012373873-8.00051-7
Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 8719–8724. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900234106
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718–731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal performance. New York: Harper and Row.
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behaviour: Theory and preliminary testing. In A. L. B. H.S. Levin, H.M. Eisenberg (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 217–229). New York: Oxford University Press.
Desmedt, O., Luminet, O., & Corneille, O. (2018). The heartbeat counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes: Evidence from both the original and an adapted counting task. Biological Psychology, 138, 185–188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.09.004
Esterman, M., Noonan, S. K., Rosenberg, M., & Degutis, J. (2013). In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cerebral Cortex, 23(11), 2712–2723. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs261
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2009). How many dual-process theories do we need? One, two, or many? In J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 33–54). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.003.0002
Evans, J. St. B. T. B. T. (2011). Dual-process theories of reasoning: Contemporary issues and developmental applications. Developmental Review, 31(2–3), 86–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007
Evans, J. St. B. T. J. S. B. T. (2012). Spot the difference: Distinguishing between two kinds of processing. Mind and Society, 11(1), 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-012-0104-2
Finnigan, F., Schulze, D., & Smallwood, J. (2007). Alcohol and the wandering mind: A new direction in the study of alcohol on attentional lapses. International Journal on Disability and Human Development, 6(2), 189–200. https://doi.org/10.1515/IJDHD.2007.6.2.189
Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(200001/03)13:1<1::AID-BDM333>3.0.CO;2-S
Fischer, D., Messner, M., & Pollatos, O. (2017). Improvement of interoceptive processes after an 8-week body scan intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00452
Fox, K. C. R., & Christoff, K. (2014). Metacognitive facilitation of spontaneous thought processes: When metacognition helps the wandering mind Find its way. In S. M. Fleming & C. D. Frith (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition (pp. 293–319). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-45190-4
Fox, K. C. R., Zakarauskas, P., Dixon, M., Ellamil, M., Thompson, E., & Christoff, K. (2012). Meditation experience predicts introspective accuracy. PLoS ONE, 7(9). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045370
Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., Anderson, C. L., Johnston, C., Smallwood, J., Kingstone, A., & Schooler, J. W. (2017). Tracking distraction: The relationship between mind-wandering, meta-awareness, and ADHD symptomatology. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(6), 475–486. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054714543494
Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., Anderson, C. L., Smallwood, J., Kingstone, A., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). The silver lining of a mind in the clouds: Interesting musings are associated with positive mood while mind-wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(August), 583. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00583
Garfinkel, S. N., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Interoception, emotion and brain: New insights link internal physiology to social behaviour. Commentary on: “Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety” by Terasawa et al. (2012). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 231–234. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss140
Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart : Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology, 104, 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004
Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10(1), 83–91. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018441
Groot, J. M., Boayue, N. M., Csifcsák, G., Boekel, W., Huster, R., Forstmann, B. U., & Mittner, M. (2021). Probing the neural signature of mind wandering with simultaneous fMRI-EEG and pupillometry. NeuroImage, 224(September 2020), 117412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117412
Han, J., Waddington, G., Adams, R., Anson, J., & Liu, Y. (2016). Assessing proprioception: A critical review of methods. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 5(1), 80–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2014.10.004
Hao, N., Wu, M., Runco, M. A., & Pina, J. (2015). More mind wandering, fewer original ideas: Be not distracted during creative idea generation. Acta Psychologica, 161, 110–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.09.001
Hawkins, G. E., Mittner, M., Boekel, W., Heathcote, A., & Forstmann, B. U. (2015). Toward a model-based cognitive neuroscience of mind wandering. Neuroscience, 310, 290–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.09.053
Herbert, B. M., Herbert, C., & Pollatos, O. (2011). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? Journal of Personality, 79(5), 1149–1175. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00717.x
Hillier, S., Immink, M., & Thewlis, D. (2015). Assessing proprioception: A systematic review of possibilities. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 29(10), 933–949. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968315573055
Hölzel, B. B. K., Lazar, S. W. S. W., Holzel, B. K., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 537–559. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611419671
Huang, F.-Y., Wu, C. W., Bhikshu, H., Shih, G., Chao, Y.-P., & Dai, C.-T. (2015). Validation of the Taiwanese Version of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (T-FFMQ). Psychological Testing, 62(3), 231–260.
Huba, G. J., Singer, J. L., Aneshensel, C. S., & Antrobus, J. S. (1982). Short imaginal processes inventory. Ann Arbor, Michigan: Research Psychologist Press.
Irish, M., Goldberg, Z. lee, Alaeddin, S., O’Callaghan, C., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2019). Age-related changes in the temporal focus and self-referential content of spontaneous cognition during periods of low cognitive demand. Psychological Research, 83(4), 747–760. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1102-8
Irving, Z. C., & Glasser, A. (2019). Mind-wandering: A philosophical guide. Philosophy Compass, 15(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12644
Irving, Z. C., Glasser, A., Gopnik, A., Pinter, V., & Sripada, C. (2020). What does “mind‐wandering” mean to the folk? An empirical investigation. Cognitive Science, 44(10), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12908
Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.2.109
Jin, C. Y., Borst, J. P., & van Vugt, M. K. (2019). Predicting task-general mind-wandering with EEG. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00707-1
Ju, Y.-J., & Lien, Y.-W. (2016). Better control with less effort: The advantage of using focused-breathing strategy over focused-distraction strategy on thought suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 9–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.002
Ju, Y.-J., & Lien, Y.-W. (2018). Who is prone to wander and when? Examining an integrative effect of working memory capacity and mindfulness trait on mind wandering under different task loads. Consciousness and Cognition, 63(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.06.006
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144–156. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpg016
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Retrieved from http://www.amazon.com/Thinking-Fast-Slow-Daniel-Kahneman/dp/0374275637
Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-germeys, I., Kwapil, T. R., & Study, A. E. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18(7), 614–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x
Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2008). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in Working Memory (pp. 21–48). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195168648.003.0002
Kane, M. J., Gross, G. M., Chun, C. A., Smeekens, B. A., Meier, M. E., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2017). For whom the mind wanders, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychological Science, 28(9), 095679761770608. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617706086
Kane, M., & Smeekens, B. (2021). Testing the construct validity of competing measurement approaches to probed mind-wandering reports. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01557-x
Kang, Y., Gruber, J., & Gray, J. R. (2013). Mindfulness and de-automatization. Emotion Review, 5(2), 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451629
Kassambara, A. (2020). ggpubr: ‘ggplot2’ Based Publication Ready Plots. R package version 0.4.0.
Kawagoe, T., & Kase, T. (2020). Task-related thought and metacognitive ability in mind wandering reports: An exploratory study. Psychological Research, (0123456789). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01346-9
Kerr, C. E., Sacchet, M. D., Lazar, S. W., Moore, C. I., & Jones, S. R. (2013). Mindfulness starts with the body: Somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(JAN), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00012
Killingsworth, M. a, & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science (New York, N.Y.), 330(6006), 932. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1192439
Klesel, M., Oschinsky, F. M., Conrad, C., & Niehaves, B. (2021). Does the type of mind wandering matter? Extending the inquiry about the role of mind wandering in the IT use experience. Internet Research. https://doi.org/10.1108/INTR-05-2020-0262
Klinger, E. (2009). Daydreaming and fantasizing: thought flow and motivation. In Keith D. Markman , William M. P. Klein & Julie A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 225–239). Abingdon: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203809846.ch15
Klinger, E., Marchetti, I., & Koster, E. H. W. (2018). Spontaneous thought and goal pursuit: From functions such as planning to dysfunctions such as rumination. In Kieran C.R. Fox & Kalina Christoff (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and dreaming. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.24
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
Levinson, D. B., Smallwood, J., & Davidson, R. J. (2012). The persistence of thought: evidence for a role of working memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated thinking. Psychological Science, 23(4), 375–380. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611431465
Long, J. A. (2019). interactions: Comprehensive, user-friendly toolkit for probing interactions. R package version 1.1.0, https://cran.r-project.org/package=interactions.
Lüdecke, D. (2020). sjPlot: Data Visualization for Statistics in Social Science. R package version 2.4.1.9000, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sjPlot.
Martin, J. R. (1997). Mindfulness: A Proposed Common Factor. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration Publication Discontinued, 7(4), 291–312. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOPI.0000010885.18025.bc
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science (New York, N.Y.), 315(5810), 393–395. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1131295
Matthews, G., Szalma, J., Panganiban, A. R., Neubauer, C., & Warm, J. S. (2013). Profiling task stress with the dundee state questionnaire. In Leandro Cavalcanti & Sofia Azevedo (Eds.), Psychology of Stress: New Research (pp. 49-91). Nova Science Publishers.
McCabe, D. P., Roediger, H. L., McDaniel, M. A., Balota, D. A., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: Evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology, 24(2), 222–243. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017619
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(1), 196–204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014104
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 188–197; discussion 198-207. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018298
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 141(2), 302–320. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025250
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2013). Dispatching the wandering mind? Toward a laboratory method for cuing “spontaneous” off-task thought. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(September), 570. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00570
McVay, J. C., Unsworth, N., McMillan, B. D., & Kane, M. J. (2013). Working memory capacity does not always support future-oriented mind-wandering. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031252
Meessen, J., Mainz, V., Gauggel, S., Volz-Sidiropoulou, E., Sütterlin, S., & Forkmann, T. (2016). The relationship between interoception and metacognition: A pilot study. Journal of Psychophysiology, 30(2), 76–86. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000157
Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E., & Stewart, A. (2012). The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE, 7(11), e48230. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048230
Meier, M. E. (2019). Is there a positive association between working memory capacity and mind wandering in a low-demand breathing task? A preregistered replication of a study by Levinson, Smallwood, And Davidson (2012). Psychological Science, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619837942
Metzinger, T. (2018). Why is mind wandering interesting for philosophers. The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming, 97–111.
Mills, C., Raffaelli, Q., Irving, Z. C., Stan, D., & Christoff, K. (2017). Is an off-task mind a freely-moving mind? Examining the relationship between different dimensions of thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 58(September), 20–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.10.003
Mitchell, J. P., Heatherton, T. F., Kelley, W. M., Wyland, C. L., Wegner, D. M., & Neil Macrae, C. (2007). Separating sustained from transient aspects of cognitive control during thought suppression. Psychological Science, 18(4), 292–297. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01891.x
Mooneyham, B. W., & Schooler, J. W. (2016). Mind wandering minimizes mind numbing: Reducing semantic-satiation effects through absorptive lapses of attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(4), 1273–1279. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0993-2
Mooneyham, B. W., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: a review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(1), 11–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031569
Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24(5), 776–781. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612459659
Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(3), 442–448. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026678
Murphy, J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2018). Alexithymia is associated with a multidomain, multidimensional failure of interoception: Evidence from novel tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(3), 398–408. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000366
Murray, S., & Krasich, K. (2020). Can the mind wander intentionally? Mind & Language, (February), mila.12332. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12332
Ortner, C. N. M., Kilner, S. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task. Motivation and Emotion, 31(4), 271–283. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9076-7
Parkin, L., Morgan, R., Rosselli, A., Howard, M., Sheppard, A., Evans, D., … Dunn, B. (2014). Exploring the relationship between mindfulness and cardiac perception. Mindfulness, 5(3), 298–313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0181-7
Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., … Lindeløv, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 195–203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y
Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
Plimpton, B., Patel, P., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2015). Role of triggers and dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A laboratory study. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 261–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.014
Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., & Miles, E. (2013). Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1412–1421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.09.012
Pollatos, O., Kurz, A.-L., Albrecht, J., Schreder, T., Kleemann, A. M., Schöpf, V., … Schandry, R. (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 9(4), 381–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.02.001
Polychroni, N., Hedman, L. R. A., & Terhune, D. B. (2020). Response time fluctuations in the sustained attention to response task predict performance accuracy and meta-awareness of attentional states. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000248
Quayle, J. D., & Ball, L. J. (2000). Working memory, metacognitive uncertainty, and belief bias in syllogistic reasoning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 53(4), 1202–1223. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755945
Raffone, A., Marzetti, L., del Gratta, C., Perrucci, M. G., Romani, G. L., Pizzella, V., & Del, C. (2019). Toward a brain theory of meditation. In Narayanan Srinivasan (Eds.), Progress in Brain Research (1st ed.). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.028
Randall, J. G., Beier, M. E., & Villado, A. J. (2019). Multiple routes to mind wandering: Predicting mind wandering with resource theories. Consciousness and Cognition, 67(August 2018), 26–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.11.006
Ritchie, J. B., & Carruthers, P. (2015). The bodily senses. In M. Matthen (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Perception (pp. 353–370). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199600472.013.026
Robison, M. K., Miller, A. L., & Unsworth, N. (2019). Examining the effects of probe frequency, response options, and framing within the thought-probe method. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 398–408. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01212-6
Robison, M. K., & Unsworth, N. (2017a). Individual differences in working memory capacity and resistance to belief bias in syllogistic reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(8), 1471–1484. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1188406
Robison, M. K., & Unsworth, N. (2017b). Working memory capacity and mind-wandering during low-demand cognitive tasks. Consciousness and Cognition, 52(February), 47–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.04.012
Robison, M. K., & Unsworth, N. (2018). Cognitive and contextual correlates of spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 44(1), 85–98. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000444
Rummel, J., & Boywitt, C. D. (2014). Controlling the stream of thought: Working memory capacity predicts adjustment of mind-wandering to situational demands. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1309–1315. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0580-3
Sayette, M. a, Reichle, E. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Lost in the sauce: The effects of alcohol on mind wandering. Psychological Science, 20(6), 747–752. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02351.x
Schlagman, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2008). Involuntary autobiographical memories in and outside the laboratory: How different are they from voluntary autobiographical memories? Memory and Cognition, 36(5), 920–932. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.5.920
Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (2004). Zoning out while reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and metaconsciousness. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children (pp. 203–226). MIT Press.
Schooler, Jonathan W. (2002). Re-representing consciousness: dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(8), 339–344. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01949-6
Schooler, Jonathan W., Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Baird, B., Mooneyham, B. W., Zedelius, C., … Schooler, J. W. (2014). The middle way : Finding the balance between mindfulness and mind-wandering. In Brian H. Ross (Eds.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (1st ed., Vol. 60, pp. 1–33). Elsevier Inc. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800090-8.00001-9
Schooler, Jonathan W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. a. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 319–326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006
Schubert, A.-L. L., Frischkorn, G. T., & Rummel, J. (2019). The validity of the online thought-probing procedure of mind wandering is not threatened by variations of probe rate and probe framing. Psychological Research, (0123456789). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01194-2
Seli, P. (2016). The Attention-Lapse and Motor Decoupling accounts of SART performance are not mutually exclusive. Consciousness and Cognition, 41, 189–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.017
Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A. A., Levene, M., & Smilek, D. (2013). How few and far between? Examining the effects of probe rate on self-reported mind wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(JUL). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00430
Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A. a, & Smilek, D. (2014). Not all mind wandering is created equal: Dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychological Research, 79(5), 750–758. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0617-x
Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., Wammes, J. D., Risko, E. F., Schacter, D. L., & Smilek, D. (2018). On the clock: Evidence for the rapid and strategic modulation of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1247–1256. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618761039
Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., Xu, M., Purdon, C., & Smilek, D. (2015). Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(2), 1417–1425. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000116
Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering minds and wavering rhythms: Linking mind wandering and behavioral variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(1), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030954
Seli, P., Jonker, T. R., Cheyne, J. A., Cortes, K., & Smilek, D. (2015). Can research participants comment authoritatively on the validity of their self-reports of mind wandering and task engagement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 703–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000029
Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., Schooler, J. W., & Smilek, D. (2018). Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(6), 479–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010
Seli, P., Ralph, B. C. W., Risko, E. F., W. Schooler, J., Schacter, D. L., & Smilek, D. (2017). Intentionality and meta-awareness of mind wandering: Are they one and the same, or distinct dimensions? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(6), 1808–1818. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1249-0
Seli, P., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016). On the necessity of distinguishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychological Science, 27(5), 0956797616634068-. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616634068
Seli, P., Schacter, D. L., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2019). Increasing participant motivation reduces rates of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychological Research, 83(5), 1057–1069. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0914-2
Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Price, J. L., Rundle, M. M., Vaishnavi, S. N., Snyder, A. Z., … Raichle, M. E. (2009). The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(6), 1942–1947. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812686106
Shepherd, J. (2019). Why does the mind wander? Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2019(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niz014
Smallwood, J. (2013). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: A process-occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. Psychological Bulletin, 139(3), 519–535. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030010
Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(August), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441
Smallwood, J., Beach, E., Schooler, J. W., & Handy, T. C. (2008). Going AWOL in the brain: Mind wandering reduces cortical analysis of external events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 458–469. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20037
Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 9(2), 271–276. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014855
Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 527–533. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194102

Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind. Memory & Cognition, 36(6), 1144–1150. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.6.1144
Smallwood, J., Nind, L., & O’Connor, R. C. (2009). When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 118–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.004
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946
Smeekens, B. A., & Kane, M. J. (2016). Working memory capacity, mind wandering, and creative cognition: An individual-differences investigation into the benefits of controlled versus spontaneous thought. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(4), 389–415. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000046
Smith, A. C., Brosowsky, N. P., Ralph, B. C. W., Smilek, D., & Seli, P. (2021). Re-examining the effect of motivation on intentional and unintentional task-unrelated thought: Accounting for thought constraint produces novel results. Psychological Research, (0123456789). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01487-5
Song, C., Kanai, R., Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Schwarzkopf, D. S., & Rees, G. (2011). Relating inter-individual differences in metacognitive performance on different perceptual tasks. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1787–1792. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.011
Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(34), 12569–12574. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800005105
Stanovich, K. E. (2009). Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: Is it time for a tri-process theory. In J. S. B. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 55–88). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.003.0003
Stanovich, Keith. (2010). Rationality and the reflective mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195341140.001.0001
Tang, Y., Hölzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3916
Tang, Y. Y., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2012). Neural correlates of establishing, maintaining, and switching brain states. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(6), 330–337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.001
Tang, Y.-Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., … Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17152–17156. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707678104
Taruffi, L., Pehrs, C., Skouras, S., & Koelsch, S. (2017). Effects of sad and happy music on mind-wandering and the default mode network. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14849-0
R core team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Teng, S. C., & Lien, Y. W. (2016). What Confucius practiced is good for your mind: Examining the effect of a contemplative practice in Confucian tradition on executive functions. Consciousness and Cognition, 42(1), 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.016
Teng, S. C., & Lien, Y. W. (under review). Propensity or diversity? –Investigating how mind-wandering influences the incubation effect of creativity.
Thompson, V. A., Turner, J. A. P., & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition , reason , and metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63(3), 107–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.06.001
Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2015). A resource-control account of sustained attention: Evidence from mind-wandering and vigilance paradigms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 82–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614556681
Unsworth, N., & McMillan, B. D. (2013). Mind wandering and reading comprehension: Examining the roles of working memory capacity, interest, motivation, and topic experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(3), 832–842. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029669
Unsworth, N., & McMillan, B. D. (2014). Similarities and differences between mind-wandering and external distraction: A latent variable analysis of lapses of attention and their relation to cognitive abilities. Acta Psychologica, 150, 14–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.04.001
Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2018). Tracking arousal state and mind wandering with pupillometry. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(4), 638–664. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0594-4
Vago, D. R., & Zeidan, F. (2016). The brain on silent: mind wandering, mindful awareness, and states of mental tranquility. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1373(1), 96–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13171
van Vugt, M. K., & Broers, N. (2016). Self-reported stickiness of mind-wandering affects task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(May), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00732
Vannucci, M., Pelagatti, C., Hanczakowski, M., Mazzoni, G., & Paccani, C. R. (2015). Why are we not flooded by involuntary autobiographical memories? Few cues are more effective than many. Psychological Research, 79(6), 1077–1085. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0632-y
Vannucci, M., Pelagatti, C., & Marchetti, I. (2017). Manipulating cues in mind wandering: Verbal cues affect the frequency and the temporal focus of mind wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 53(February), 61–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.06.004
Varao-Sousa, T. L., & Kingstone, A. (2019). Are mind wandering rates an artifact of the probe-caught method? Using self-caught mind wandering in the classroom to test, and reject, this possibility. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 235–242. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1073-0
Ward, A. F. A., & Wegner, D. M. D. (2013). Mind-blanking: When the mind goes away. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(September), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00650
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101(1), 34–52. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/101/1/34/
Weinstein, Y. (2017). Are you mind-wandering , or is your mind on task ? The effect of probe framing on mind- wandering reports. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, (May). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1322-8
Welhaf, M. S., Smeekens, B. A., Gazzia, N. C., Perkins, J. B., Silvia, P. J., Meier, M. E., … Kane, M. J. (2020). An exploratory analysis of individual differences in mind wandering content and consistency. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7(2), 103–125. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000180
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer New York. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-98141-3
Wölk, J., Sütterlin, S., Koch, S., Vögele, C., & Schulz, S. M. (2014). Enhanced cardiac perception predicts impaired performance in the Iowa Gambling Task in patients with panic disorder. Brain and Behavior, 4(2), 238–246. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.206
Ye, Q., Song, X., Zhang, Y., & Wang, Q. (2014). Children’s mental time travel during mind wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(August), 927. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00927
Zamariola, G., Maurage, P., Luminet, O., & Corneille, O. (2018). Interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task are problematic: Evidence from simple bivariate correlations. Biological Psychology, 137(June), 12–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.06.006
Zavagnin, M., Borella, E., De Beni, R., Beni, R. De, De Beni, R., M., Z., … E., B. (2014). When the mind wanders: Age-related differences between young and older adults. Acta Psychologica, 145(1), 54–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.10.016
Zhou, Z.-W., Lan, X.-Q., Fang, Y.-T., Gong, Y., Zang, Y.-F., Luo, H., & Zhang, H. (2019). The inter-regional connectivity within the default mode network during the attentional processes of internal focus and external focus: An fMRI study of continuous finger force feedback. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(September), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02198
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
QR Code
QRCODE