:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:中文讀者對於中文字構形規則的知識:一個統計取向之語言學習理論的研究
作者:羅明
作者(外文):Ming Lo
校院名稱:臺灣大學
系所名稱:心理學研究所
指導教授:胡志偉
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2011
主題關鍵詞:統計取向學習閱讀中文字學習事件誘發電位文字辨識statistical learningreadingChinese character learningevent-related potentialsvisual word recognition
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(0) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:45
本研究旨在以統計取向的學習觀探討中文字的習得。統計取向的學習觀主張,任何語言刺激的背後皆有一個統計性的特徵,而一般人天生有能力自發的掌握該特徵,並將其轉化為一種規則。中文字在絕大多數的情況下是由兩個部件所組成,其中又以左右橫排的結構最為常見,而且能夠提供字音線索的部件(簡稱「聲旁」),通常是位於一個字的右半邊的部件。根據統計取向的學習觀,中文讀者在學習中文字的過程中,將會發覺聲旁有很高的機率是一個字的右部件,並據以發展出「聲旁應該是字的右部件」的規則。為了觀察個體在學習發展的過程中可能產生的改變,本研究除了邀請一般的大學生,也邀請了小學低與高年級的學童擔任本研究的受試者。為了瞭解中文讀者學習中文字時所發展出的規則與文字之形音處理歷程的關係,本研究的實驗刺激包含了受試者未曾接觸過的假字,以及日常生活中可能會接觸到的用字,然後請受試者對這些實驗刺激進行兩類涉及語音處理的作業。其中一類作業要求受試者默唸刺激字的發音,然後記錄發音的內容,以及受試者默唸時刺激字誘發的事件相關電位(event-related potentials),另一類作業則要求受試者直接朗讀出刺激字的發音,然後記錄發音的正確性與反應速度。實驗結果發現,受試者會表現出一種以刺激字右部件判斷刺激字發音的傾向,而且這個傾向在大學生與國小高年級學童的反應中尤其明顯,顯示中文讀者對於聲旁在字中的空間位置,在國小的階段就發展出某種抽象的規則。除此之外,不論刺激字屬於真字或假字,當刺激字內的聲旁出現在不符合規則的位置時,腦波成分P200在事件相關電位的波形上的振幅會顯著的增大,但是當受試者是國小低年級學童時,P200的振幅較不會因刺激字的種類而有所變化。而對於朗讀刺激字的行為反應,不符合規則的刺激字會讓大學生與國小高年級學童表現出較慢的反應速度,顯示著處理歷程受到某種抑制的作用。綜合而言,本研究的結果顯示,當中文讀者學習了一定數量的中文字之後,他們對於聲旁的空間位置會發展出一種主觀性的規則。而且一如統計取向學習觀的預期,關於聲旁空間位置的規則反映著語言刺激中最常見的例子,也就是右部件是一個聲旁的字。
Learning of Chinese characters is described in terms of learning and regularization of a statistical structure underlying linguistic inputs (Shaffran, 2003; Wonnacott & Newport, 2005). The majority of Chinese characters are LR-characters, in which a character consists of two components that are arraged to form a left-and-right structure. For instance, the character ‘棋’ /qi2 / is composed of the components ‘木’ /mu4/ and ‘其’ /qi2/, and another character ‘期’ /qi2/ consists of the components ‘其’ /qi2/ and ‘月’ /yue4/. Although the component that hints the pronunciation of a LR-character can be the right or the left component of the character, the phonetic component is frequently the right component of a LR-character. If, after learning an amount of characters, a Chinese reader tends to regularize the position of a phonetic component of a LR-character to develop a rule that ‘其’ should be the right part of a LR-character, then ‘棋’ is the one that follows the rule while ‘期’ is the one that violates the rule, and the two types of characters should be treated differently by the reader. To investigate the relationship between the number of characters a reader has learned and the the way that a reader develops rules of the position of phonetic components, a group of college and elementary school students was invited to participate in the present research. In addition, to investigate what is the effect of the rules on the cognitive processing of a LR-character, the participants were asked to read a number of real characters or pseudo-characters. In Exp. 1 and 2, ten college students and twelve first-graders were asked to sub-vocalize pseudo-characters. Event-related potentials were measured, and greater P200 amplitudes were elicited in the condition that the left component of a pseudo-character was a phonetic component. However, the effect was not found for the first-graders. In the third and the fourth experiments, 27 college students and 24 fifth-graders were asked to sub-vocalize regular and irregular characters. Event-related potentials were also measured, and for both groups of the participants, greater P200 amplitudes were elicited when the character is regular and the left component of the character was a phonetic component. In Exp. 5 and 6, twenty college students and thirty sixth-graders were asked to read aloud regular and irregular characters. The regularity effect in Chinese (Hue, 1992; Lee, Tsai, Su, Tzeng, & Hung, 2005) was replicated, and reaction times lengthened in the condition that the left component of a regular character was a phonetic component. The pattern of the results indicates that the joint probability between the structure of characters and the position of a phonetic component is acquired and regularized by readers to form an abstract rule of orthographic structure of LR-characters. And Chinese young readers are able to form the abstract rule of orthographic structure of LR-characters when they are in fifth grade.
中文詞知識庫小組(1993)。《新聞語料詞頻統計表》。台北:中央研究院資訊科學研究所中文詞知識庫小組。

國立編譯館(1995)。《國民小學國語課本》。台北:國立編譯館。

國語推行委員會(2000)。《國小學童常用字詞調查報告書》。台北:教育部。

Adrian, E. D., & Matthews, B. H. C. (1934). The Berger rhythm: Potential changes from the occipital lobes in man. Brain, 57, 355-385.

Arciuli, J., & Cupples, L. (2006). The processing of lexical stress during visual word recognition: Typicality effects and orthographic correlates. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 920-948.

Arciuli, J., Monaghan, P., & Seva, N. (2010). Learning to assign lexical stress during reading aloud: Corpus, behavioral, and computational investigations. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 180-196.

Baayen, R., Piepenbrock, R., & van Riun, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.

Balaguer, R. D., Toro, J. M., Rodriguez-Fornell, A., & Bachoud-Lévl, A. C. (2007). Different Neurophysiological Mechanisms Underlying Word and Rule Extraction from Speech. Plos One, 2, e1175.

Bauman, D. (2008). Open your eyes: Deaf studies talking. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams Syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Supplement, 6, 115-125.

Boas, F. (1931). Race and progress. Science, 74, 1-8.

Brooks, L. R., & Vokey, J. R. (1991). Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 316-323.

Bruck, M., Hebert, M., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1984). Acquisition and use of spelling-sound correspondences in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 114-133.

Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., & Barber, H. (2005). Early event-related potential effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1803-1817.

Chan, L., & Nunes, T. (1998). Children’s understanding of the formal and functional characteristics of written Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 115-131.

Chen, Y. P., & Allport, A. (1995). Attention and lexical decomposition in Chinese word recognition: Conjunctions of form and position guide selective attention. Visual Cognition, 2, 235-268.

Chen, Y. P., Allport, D. A., & Marshall, J. C. (1996). What are the functional orthographic units in Chinese word recognition: The stroke or the stroke pattern? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 1024-1043.

Cheng, C. M. (1987). Perception of Chinese characters. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 23, 137-153.

Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner’s “Verbal Behavior.” Language, 35, 26-58.

Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A, & Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: News from the front. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 406-416.

Content, A. (1991). The effect of spelling-to-sound regularity on naming in French. Psychological Research, 53, 3-12.

Cunillera, T., Laine, M., Camara, E., Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2010). Bridging the gap between speech segmentation and word-to-world mappings: Evidence from an audiovisual statistical learning task. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 295-305.

Cunillera, T., Toro, J. M., Sebastian-Galles, N., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2006). The effects of stress and statistical cues on continuous speech segmentation: An event-related brain potential study. Brain Research, 1123, 168-178.

Defrancis, J. (1984). The Chinese language: Fact and fantasy. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.

Fang, S. P., Horng, R. Y., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (1986). Consistency effects in the Chinese character and pseudo-character naming tasks. In H. S. R. Gao & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Linguistics, psychology, and the Chinese Language (pp. 11-21). Hong Kong: University of Hong Kong, Center of Asian Studies.

Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1997). The role of component function in visual recognition of Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 23, 776-781.

Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1999). Semantic radicals contribute to the visual identification of Chinese characters. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 559-576.

Fiser, J., & Aslin, R. N. (2001). Unsupervised statistical learning of higher-order spatial structures from visual scenes. Psychological Science, 12, 499-504.

Fiser, J., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 15822-15826.

Fiser, J., & Aslin, R. N. (2005). Encoding multielement scenes: Statistical learning of visual feature hierarchies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 521-537.

Gibbs, F. A., Davis, H., & Lennox, W. G. (1935). The electro-encephalogram in epilepsy and in conditions of impaired consciousness. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 34, 1133-1148.

Gibson, E. J., Pick, A., Hammond, M., & Osser, H. (1962). Role of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in perception of words. American Journal of Psychology, 75, 554-570.

Goldin-Meadon, S., & Mylander, C. (1984). Gestural communication in deaf children: The non-effects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49, 1-151.

Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. (1991). Familiar aggregation of a genetic language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1-50.

Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & de Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19-52.

Hillyard, S. A. (1985). Electrophysiology of human selective attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 400-405.

Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (2000). Effects of word frequency and spelling-to-sound regularity in naming with and without preceding lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 166-183.

Ho, C. S. H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Learning to read Chinese beyond the logographic phase. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 276-289.

Ho, C. S. H., Ng, T. T., & Ng, W. K. (2003). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35, 849-878.

Hsiao, J. H. W., & Shillcock, R. (2004). Connectionist modeling of Chinese character pronunciation based on foveal splitting, Proceedings of the Twenty Sixth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 2004, pp. 601-606.

Hsiao, J. H. W., & Shillcock, R. (2005a). Foveal splitting causes differential processing of Chinese orthography in the male and female brain. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 531-536.

Hsiao, J. H. W., & Shillcock, R. (2005b). Differences of split and non-split architechtures emerged from modeling Chinese character pronunciation, Proceedings of the Twenty Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 2005, pp. 989-994.

Hsiao, J. H. W., Shillcock, R., & Lee, C. Y. (2007). Neural correlates of foveal splitting in reading: Evidence from an ERP study of Chinese character recognition. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1280-1292.

Hsu, C. H., Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., & Tzeng, Oz J. L. (2009). Orthographic combinability and phonological consistency effects in reading Chinese phonograms: An event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 108, 56-66.

Hue, C. W. (1992). Recognition processing in character naming. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 93-107). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Hue, C. W. (2003). Number of characters a college student knows. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 31, 300-339.

Hue, C. W., & Tzeng, A. K. Y. (2002). Lexical recognition task: A new method for the study of Chinese character recognition. Psychologia Sinica, 32, 60-65.

Jasper, H. H. & Carmichael, L. (1935). Electrical potentials from the intact human brain. Science, 81, 51-53.

Kelly, M. H., Morris, J., & Verrekia, L. (1998). Orthographic cues to lexical stress: Effects on naming and lexical decision. Memory and Cognition, 26, 822-832.

Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83, B35-B42.

Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., Richardson, D. C., & Johnson, S. P. (2007). Location, location, location: Development of spatiotemporal sequence learning in infancy, Child Development, 78, 1559-1571.

Koester, D. & Prinz, W. (2007). Capturing regularities in event sequences: Evidence for two mechanisms. Brain Research, 1180, 59-77.

Kong, L. Y., Zhang, J. X., Kang, C. P., Du, Y. C., Zhang, B., & Wang, S. P. (2010). P200 and phonological processing in Chinese word recognition. Neuroscience Letters, 473, 37-41.

Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647.

Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315-334.

Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Chiu, Y. C., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hung, D. L. (2006). The early extraction of sublexical phonology in reading Chinese pseudocharacters: An event-related potentials study. Language and Linguistics, 7, 619-635.

Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Su, E. C. I., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hung, D. L. (2005). Consistency, regularity, and frequency effects in naming Chinese characters. Language and Linguistics, 6, 75-107.

Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Chan, W. H., Hsu, C. H., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2007). Temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: An event-related potentials study. Neuroreport, 18, 147-151.

Liao, C. H., Geogiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2008). Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition. Reading & Writing, 21, 231-253.

Liu, P. D., Chung, K. K. H., McBride-Chang, C., & Tong, X. H. (2010). Holistic versus analytic processing: evidence for a different approach to processing of Chinese at the word and character levels in Chinese children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 466-478.

Liu, B. L., Jin, Z. X., Qing, Z., & Wang, Z. N. (2011). The processing of phonological, orthographical, and lexical information of Chinese characters in sentence contexts: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1372, 81-91.

Liu, B. L., Jin, Z. X., Wang, Z. N., & Xin, S. A. (2011). An ERP study on whether the P600 can reflect the presence of unexpected phonology. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 399-408.

Liu, Y., Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2003). ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory & Cognition, 29, 1231-1247.

Lo, M., & Hue, C. W. (2008). C-CAT: A computer software used to analyze and select Chinese characters and character components for psychology research. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 1098-1105.

Lo, M., Hue, C. W., & Tsai, F. Z. (2007). Chinese readers’ knowledge of how Chinese orthography represents phonology. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 49, 315-334.

Lo, M., Hue, C. W., & Tzeng, Y. H. (in press). C-CAT2: A computer software used to analyze traditional and simplified Chinese characters, character components and neighbors. Chinese Journal of Psychology.

Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Marcovitch, S. & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2009). Sequence learning in infancy: the independent contributions of conditional probability and pair frequency information. Developmental Science, 12, 1020-1025.

Massaro, D. W., Venezky, R. L., & Taylor, G. A. (1979). Orthographic regularity, positional frequency, and visual processing of letter strings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 107-124.

Mathews, R. C., Buss, R. R., Stanley, W. B., Blanchardfields, F., Cho, J. R., & Druhan, B. (1989). Role of implicit and explicit process in learning from examples: A synergistic effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1083-1100.

Meng, X. Z., Jian, J., Shu, H., Tian, X. M., & Zhou, X. L. (2008). ERP correlates of the development of orthographical and phonological processing during Chinese sentence reading. Brain Research, 1219, 91-102.

Miller, G. A., Bruner, J. S., & Postman, L. (1954). Familiarity of letter sequences and tachistoscopic identification. The Journal of General Psychology, 50, 129-139.

Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognition Science, 14, 11-28.

Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. (1985). The acquisition of American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and noneffects of maternal speech style. In C. Snow & C. Ferguson, Eds., Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Paulesu, E., Demonet, J. F., Fzxio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cossu, G., Habib, M., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2004). Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 27165-2167.

Perruchet, P., & Pacton, S. (2006). Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 233-238.

Perruchet, P., & Pacteau, C. (1990). Synthetic grammar learning: Implicit rule abstraction or explicit fragmentary knowledge? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 264-275.

Perruchet, P., Vinter, A., & Gallego, J. (1997). Implicit learning shapes new conscious percepts and representations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 43-48.

Picton, T. W. (1992). The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 9, 456-479.

Pinker, S., (1994). The language instinct. New Your: Morrow.

Reali, F., & Griffiths, T. L. (2009). The evolution of frequency distributions: Relating regularization to inductive biases through iterated learning. Cognition, 111, 317-328.

Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855-863.

Reber, A. S. (1997). Implicit ruminations, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 49-55.

Regan, D. (1989). Human brain electrophysiology: Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine. New York: Elsevier.

Rugg, M. D. (1990). Event-related brain potentials dissociate repetition effects of high-grequency and low-frequency words. Memory & Cognition, 18, 367-379.

Saffran, J. R. (2003). Statistical language learning: Mechanisms and constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 110-114.

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 27-52.

Saffran, J., Hauser, M., Seibel, R., Kapfhamer, J.Tsao, F., & Cushman, F. (2008). Grammatical pattern learning by human infants and cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Cognition, 107, 489-500.

Sanders, L. D., Ameral, V., & Sayles, K. (2009). Event-related potentials index segmentation of nonsense sounds. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1183-1186.

Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). The time course of phonological activation in two writing system. Cognition, 19, 1-30.

Shanks, D. R. (2010). Learning: From association to cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 273-301.

Shillcock, R., & Monaghan, P. (2001). The computational exploration of visual word recognition in a split model. Neural Computation, 13, 1171-1198.

Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Wu, N. (2000). Phonetic awareness: Knowledge of orthography–phonology relationships in the character acquisition of Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 56–62.

Singleton, J. L., & Newport, E. L. (2004). When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 370-407.

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Spoehr, K. T. & Smith, E. E. (1975). Role of orthographic and phonotatic rules in perceiving letter patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 21-34.

Stanovich, K. E., & Bauer, D. W. (1978). Experiments on spelling-to-sound regularity effect in word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 6, 410-415.

Taft, M. & Zhu, X. (1997). Submorphemic processing in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23, 761-775.

Taft, M., Zhu, X. P., & Peng, D. L. (1999). Positional specificity of radicals in Chinese character recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 498-519.

Tsai, J. L., Ten, M. H., & Wang, C. A. (2005). Recoding on Eye Movements and its Application on Chinese Reading. Research in Applied Psychology, 29, 91-104.

Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Yen, N. S. (2004). Use of phonological codes for Chinese characters: Evidence from processing of parafoveal preview when reading sentences. Brain & Language, 91, 235-244.

Tzeng, O. J., Lin, Z. H., Hung, D. L., & Lee, W. L. (1995). Learning to be a conspirator: A tale of becoming a good Chinese reader. In B. de Gelder & J. Morais (Eds.), Speech and reading: A comparative approach (pp. 227-246). Hove, England UK: Erlbaum (Uk) Taylor & Francis, Publishers.

Valdessosa, M., Gonzalez, A., Xiang, L., Zhang, X. L., Yi, H., & Bobes, M. A. (1993). Brain potentials in a phonological matching task using Chinese characters. Neuropsychologia, 31, 853-864.

Vokey, J. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1992). Salience of item knowledge in learning artificial grammar. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 328-344.

Wang, C. C., Hung, L. Y., Chang, Y., W., & Chen, H. F. (2008). Number of characters school students know from grade 1 to G9. Bulletin of Education Psychology, 39, 555-568.

Wang, M. Y., & Ching, C. L. (2009). Recognition intent and visual word recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 65-77.

Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387-407.

Wonnacott, E., & Newport, E. L., (2005). In A. Brugos, M.R. Clark-Cotton, and S. Ha (Eds.), BUCLD 29: Proceedings of the 29th Annual boston university conference on language development. MA: Cascadilla Press, Somerville.

Wu, X. C., Li, W. L., & Anderson, R. C. (1999). Reading instruction in China. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 31, 571-586.

Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. Brewster, NY: Touchstond Applied Science Associates.

Zhang, Q., Zhqng, J. X., & Kong, L. Y. (2009). An ERP study on the time course of phonological and semantic activation in Chinese word recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73, 235-245.


 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top