:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:性別刻板印象威脅對國中學生科學興趣與科學表現之關聯
作者:陳宜伶
作者(外文):Yi-Ling Chen
校院名稱:國立中山大學
系所名稱:教育研究所
指導教授:莊雪華
鄭英耀
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2018
主題關鍵詞:科學成就刻板印象威脅性別差異科學興趣刻板印象窒息stereotype threatstereotype chokinggender differencescience interestscience achievement
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(0) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:0
本研究旨在(一) 驗證性別刻板印象威脅理論三要素中,國二生的領域認同、性別刻板印象信念、教室情境性別刻板印象與國三理化興趣、理化成就的關係;(二)探究不同程度的領域認同、性別刻板印象信念、教室性別刻板印象氛圍之組合與理化興趣和理化成就之間的關係。研究資料為鄭、鍾(2007,2008)國科會計畫所蒐集的縱貫資料。從國二追蹤到高一共475人(男243,女242)。研究結果顯示(一)對於女生而言,國二的理化領域認同和性別刻板印象信念可以解釋國三理化成就,然而,僅國二理化領域認同可以解釋國三理化興趣;(二)對於男生而言,僅國二的理化領域認同可解釋國三理化成就,而國二理化領域認同和性別刻板印象信念國三理化興趣;(三)不論男女,理化領域認同在理化成就和理化興趣上扮演重要的角色;(四)對於女生而言,國三理化興趣為國二領域認同和國三理化成就之完全中介變項,而對男生而言,國三理化興趣為部分中介變項;(五)具有高領域認同、高教室情境刻板印象、低刻板印象信念的女學生理化成就最高,但其國三理化興趣卻是女生中降低最多的一組;(六)對於高領域認同女學生而言,具有高刻板印象信念的成就表現較低刻板印象信念者低,意指這群學生易受刻板印象威脅之影響;(七)對於低領域認同男學生而言,具有高刻板印象信念者或感知高教室情境刻板印象者的成就表現較低刻板印象信念者或感知低教室情境刻板印象者為低,意指這群學生易受刻板印象窒息之影響。
The study aimed (1) to examine how the three components in gender stereotype threat theory—domain identification, gender stereotype belief, and classroom gender stereotype climate—account for junior high school students’ interest and achievements measured one year later in the field of physical sciences (physics and chemistry); (2) to explore the correlations of combination of different level of domain identification, gender stereotype belief, and classroom gender stereotype climate with achievement and interest. A longitudinal data of Cheng & Chung (2007, 2008) were employed in the study. Students were tracked from grade 8 to grade 10. A total of 475 junior high school students were included in the data (Male = 243, Female =232). The results showed that for females, 8th-grade domain identification and gender stereotype belief accounted for 9th-grade physical science achievements, whereas only 8th-grade domain identification accounted for 9th-grade physical science interest. However, for males, only 8th-grade domain identification accounted for 9th-grade physical science achievements, while 8th-grade domain identification and gender stereotype accounted for 9th-grade physical science interest. Domain identification played a major role for predicting physical science achievement and interest both for males and females. Ninth-year physical science interest acts as a full mediator between 8th-grade domain identification and 9th-grade physical achievement for females, while it serves as a partial mediator for males. Female students with high domain identification, high classroom gender stereotype climate, but low gender stereotype belief had the highest 9th-grade performance but were also the group whose interest decreased most in grade 9 among other female groups. For females highly identified with physical science identification, students with low gender stereotype belief performed better than those with high gender stereotype belief, suggesting those girls were vulnerable to stereotype threat. For males with low physical science identification, students with low gender stereotype belief or perceiving classroom gender stereotype climate, indicating those boys were vulnerable to stereotype choking. Suggestions for possible educational interventions were also offered in the study.
References
Aguilar, L., Walton, G., & Wieman, C. (2014). Psychological insights for improved physics teaching. Physics Today, 67, 43-49.
Aronson, J. & Steele, C.M. (2005). Stereotypes and the fragility of human competence, motivation, and self-concept. In C. Dweck & E. Elliot (Eds.), Handbook of Competence & Motivation. New York, Guilford.
Aronson, J., Lustina, M., Good, C., Keough, K., Brown, J. L., & Steele, C. M. (1999). When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 11–23. Doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1371
Beasley, M. A., & Fischer, M. J. (2012). Why they leave: The impact of stereotype threat on the attrition of women and minorities from science, math, and engineering majors. Social Psychology of Education, 15, 427-448. Doi: 10.1007/s11218-0120-9185-3
Ceci, S., Ginther, D., Kahn, S., & Williams, W. (2015). Women in science: The path to progress. Scientific American Mind, 26(1), 62-69.
Chatard, A., Selimbegovic, L., Kona, P., & Mugny, G. (2008). Performance boosts in the classroom: Stereotype endorsement and prejudice moderate stereotype lift. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1421-1424. Doi: 10.1016/j.jesp. 2008.05.004
Cheng, Y. Y., & Chung, S. H. (2007). Influence of gender stereotype threat on female students’ learning in science classes. National Science Council (NSC-96-2522- S-110-001).
Cheng, Y. Y., & Chung, S. H. (2008). A longitudinal examination of gender stereotype threat effect mechanisms on female students’ learning in science classes. National Science Council (NSC-97-2511-S-110-004-MY2).
Cheryan, S. (2012). Understanding the paradox in math-related fields : why some gender gap remain while others do not? Sex roles, 66, 184-190. Doi: 10.1007/ s11199-011- 0060-z
Flore P. C., & Wicherts, J. M. (2015). Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 53, 25-44. Doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002
Garcia, J., Cohen, G. L. (2012). A social psychological approach to educational intervention. In Shafir, E. (Ed.), Behavioral Foundations of Policy (pp. 329-350). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Johnson, H. J., Barnard-Brak, L., Saxon, T. F., & Johnson, M. K. (2012). An experimental study of the effects of stereotype threat and stereotype lift on men and women’s performance in mathematics. The Journal of Experimental Education, 80(2), 137-149.
Keller, J. (2007). Stereotype threat in classroom settings: The interactive effect of domain identification, task difficulty and stereotype threat on female students’ math performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(2), 323-338. Doi: 10. 1348/000709906X113662
Kurtz-Costes, B., Copping, K. E., Rowley, S. J., & Kinlaw, C. R. (2014). Gender and age differences in awareness and endorsement of gender stereotypes about academic abilities. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29, 603-618. Doi: 10.1007/s10212-014-0216-7
Lane, K. A., Goh, J. X., & Driver-Linn, E. (2012). Implicit science stereotypes mediate the relationship between gender and academic participation. Sex roles, 66, 220-234. Doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0036-z
Lavonen, J., Gedrovics, J., Byman, R., Meisalo, V., Juuti, K., Uitto, A. (2008). Students’ motivational orientations and career choice in science and technology: A comparative investigation in Finland and Latvia. Journal of Baltic Science Education, 7(2), 86-102.
Leslie, S.-J., Cimpan, A., Meyer, M., & Freeland, E. (2015). Expectations of brilliance underlie gender distributions across academic disciplines. Science, 347(6219), 262-265. Doi: 10.1126/science.1261375
MacKinnon, D. P., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. H. (1995). A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivariate behavioral research, 30(1), 41-62.
Martin, M.O., Mullis, I.V.S., Foy, P., & Stanco, G.M. (2012). TIMSS 2011 International Results in Science. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College. Retrieved from http://timssandpirls.bc.edu/timss2011/international- results-science.html
Ministry of Education, Department of Statistics. (2016a). Number of college graduates. Retrieved from https://stats.moe.gov.tw/qframe.aspx?qno=MgAwADgA0
Ministry of Education, Department of Statistics. (2016b). Number of college professors. Retrieved from https://stats.moe.gov.tw/qframe.aspx?qno=MgAxADQA0
Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L. E., Finkelstein, N. D., Pollock, S. J., Cohen, G. L., & Ito, T. A. (2010). Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A classroom study of values affirmation. Science, 330(6008), 1234-1237. doi: 10.1126/science. 1195996
Murphy, M. C., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological Science, 18(10), 879-885. Doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01995.x
National Science Foundation, National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics. (2015). Women, Minorities, and Persons with Disabilities in Science and Engineering: 2015. Special Report NSF 15-311. Arlington, VA. Retrieved from http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/2015/nsf15311/
Nguyen, H.-H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1314-1334. Doi: 10.1037/a0012702
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Linner, N. M., Devos, T. D., Ayala, A…, Greenwald, A. (2009). National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Science, 106(26), 10593-10597. Doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809921106
O’Brien, L. T., & Crandall, C. S. (2003). Stereotype threat and arousal: Effects on women’s math performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 782–789. Doi: 10.1177/0146167203029006010
OECD (2007a). PISA 2006: Science competences for tomorrow’s world. (Volume 1: Analysis). PISA, OECD. Retrieved from http://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/docserver/ download/9807011e.pdf?expires=1463273726&id=id&accname=guest&checksum=01BB3E102D1EB54A32123411508FD4FC
OECD. (2007b). PISA 2006: Volume II: Data. PISA, OECD. Retrieved from http://www.nbbmuseum.be/doc/seminar2010/nl/bibliografie/opleiding/data.pdf
OECD. (2015). The ABC of gender equality in education: Aptitude, behaviour, confidence. PISA, OECD. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264229 945-en
Owens, J., Massey, D. S., (2011) Stereotype threat and college academic performance: A latent variables approach. Social Science Research, 40, 150-166. Doi:10.1016/j.ssre search.2010.09.010
Picho, K., Rodriguez, A. & Finnie, L. (2013). Exploring the moderating role of context on the mathematics performance of females under stereotype threat: A meta-analysis. The Journal of Social Psychology, 153(3), 299-333. Doi: 10.1080/00224545.2012.73 7380
Rosenthal, H. E. S., & Crisp, R. J. (2007). Choking under pressure: When an additional positive stereotype affects performance for domain identified male mathematics students. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22(3), 317-326. Doi: 10.1007 /BF03173429
Rydell, R. J., Shiffrin, R. M., Boucher, K. L., Van Loo, K., & Rydell, M. T. (2010). Stereotype threat prevents perceptual learning. PNAS, 107(32), 14042-14047.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 50, 835-850. Doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.00000 29101.74557.a0
Smith, J. L. (2004). Understanding the process of stereotype threat: A review of mediational variables and new performance goal directions. Educational Psychology Review, 16(3), 177-206. Doi: 10.1023/B:EDPR.0000034020.20317.89
Smith, J. L., & Johnson, C. (2006). A stereotype boost or choking under pressure? Positive gender stereotype and men who are low in domain identification. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28(1), 51-63. Doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp 2801_5
Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examination of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nullified stereotypes on mathematical performance: It’s not just a woman’s issue. Sex Roles, 47, 179–191. Doi: 10.1023/A:1021051223441
Smith, J. L., Sansone, C., & White, P. H. (2007). The stereotyped task engagement process: The role of interest and achievement motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(1), 99-114.
Spencer, S. J., Logel, C., & Davies, P. G. (2016). Stereotype threat. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 415-437. Doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women''s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4-28.
Steele, C. M. & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797-811. Doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotype shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613-629.
Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi: And Other Clues to How Stereotypes Affect Us. New York: Norton.
Steele, J., James, J.B., & Barnett, R.C. (2002). Learning in a man''s world: examining the perceptions of undergraduate women in male-dominated academic areas. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 46-50. Doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.00042
Wang, M.-T., & Degol, J. L. (2017). Gender gap in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM): current knowledge, implications for practice, policy, and future directions. Educational psychology review, 29(1), 119-140.
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2003). Stereotype Lift. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 456-467. Doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00019-2
Walton, G. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2009). Latent ability: Grades and test Scores systematically underestimate the intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1132-1139. Doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280. 2009.02417.x
洪秀珍、謝臥龍、駱慧文 (2015)。科技大學女學生「數學領域認同」、「數學性別刻板」、「性別角色刻板」、「情境訊息」與「數學焦慮」之研究。科學與人文研究,3(3),30-54。
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
QR Code
QRCODE