:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:本地及新移民母親教養行為特性與幼兒回應母親要求之追蹤研究
作者:李麗雯
作者(外文):Li-Wen Lee
校院名稱:國立中正大學
系所名稱:心理學所
指導教授:黃世琤
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2009
主題關鍵詞:教養新移民幼兒new immigranttoddlerparenting
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(0) 博士論文(1) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:39
本研究有兩個目的,第一是以追蹤方式探討五十四組本地、大陸與東南亞籍母親對幼兒的教養與互動行為以及幼兒行為反應之差異,第二是以階層迴歸分析追蹤家庭變項、母親的教養行為與親子互動行為對幼兒完成作業反應與回應母親要求之行為的關聯性。研究分別於幼兒十六個月與二十二個月時,測量母親在實驗室情境互動中讚許、重述、提問、指稱和肢體介入等教養行為以及親子目光接觸與分享式注意之互動行為,以及幼兒在收拾玩具時完成作業與回應母親要求之行為。以家庭社經地位與母親年齡為家庭變項,母親教養與親子互動行為分別以控制家庭變項進行共變數分析,結果顯示大陸與東南亞籍母親指稱行為顯著低於本地母親。追蹤交互作用發現,在十六個月時,東南亞母親與大陸籍母親之指稱行為均低於本地母親。此外,東南亞與大陸籍母親與其子女互動時之分享式注意行為皆低於本地家庭。有關幼兒行為之重複量數變異數分析結果顯示,幼兒獨力完成作業反應隨月齡增加,幼兒在母親協助下完成作業之反應隨月齡下降。接著以家庭變項(步驟一)與母親教養行為(步驟二)進行十六個月幼兒獨力完成行為之階層迴歸分析,結果發現,當排除家庭變項之解釋力,母親教養行為預測幼兒當時獨力完成行為,其中母親威脅行為顯著預測幼兒獨力完成行為。以家庭變項(步驟一)、幼兒十六個月之反應(步驟二)與母親教養行為(步驟三) 進行幼兒二十二個月行為之階層迴歸分析,結果發現十六個月幼兒獨力完成作業反應對於二十二個月幼兒的獨力完成行為具顯著預測力。當排除家庭變項及十六個月幼兒反應之解釋力時,母親教養行為對於十六至二十二個月中幼兒的獨力完成表現之變化具有預測力,其中,母親重述幼兒口語的教養行為顯著預測幼兒十六至二十二個月間獨力完成作業表現之變化。另一方面,當排除家庭變項之解釋力時,十六個月的幼兒行為對二十二個月幼兒不理會與強烈反抗母親要求行為具顯著預測力,其中,幼兒不理會與直接否決母親要求之行為預測幼兒二十二個月時不理會與強烈反抗母親要求的行為。因此,本地與新移民母親在子女年幼時表現之口語教養行為以及親子分享式注意即有顯著差異,母親重述幼兒口語的教養行為有促進幼兒獨力完成行為的效果。
Two major purposes of the present research was, first, to follow up fifty-four mother-toddlers dyads in two types of new immigrant families, i.e. mothers from Mainland China and Southeastern Asia, as well as in the local families, and the second, to investigate the relationship between maternal parenting behaviors and young children’s responses toward mother’s demands. The research procedures were conducted when toddlers were at 16- and 22-months old. In a free play session, the frequencies of maternal parenting behaviors such as praising the child, repeating children’s verbal responses, questioning, naming objects, threatening, and physical interruptions were videotaped and coded. In addition, mother-toddler eye gazes and joint-attentional behaviors were also measured. In a clean-up session, children’s responses of completing task independently, with mothers’ assistance, with neglect, with avoidance, or with refusal behaviors toward mothers’ demands were measured. Familial SES and mothers’ age were served as family variables. The covariate analysis was conducted to control family SES and the mother’s age. Results found a main effect of mothers’ naming objects behaviors were less in mothers from Mainland China and Southeastern than those of local mothers. The significant interaction effect and its follow-up analysis suggested that at 16 months, less naming objects were found in those mothers from Mainland China and Southeastern Asia than those local mothers. The mother-toddler joint-attentional behaviors of mothers from Mainland China and Southeastern Asia were both less than those with Taiwanese mothers. With increased age, children increased their frequencies in independent completion of the task, and their needs in mothers’ assistance decreased. The results of hierarchical regression analysis with family background (step 1) and mothers’ parenting behavior (step 2) suggested mothers’ parenting behavior predicted children’s independent completion behaviors at 16 months, that maternal threatening behavior predicted children’s independent completion behaviors. The results of hierarchical regression analysis with family background (step 1), children’s responses at 16 months (step 2), and mothers’ parenting behavior (step 3) suggested children’s independent completion behaviors at 16 months predicted children’s responses at 22 months when eliminating the effects of variables of family background. Furthermore, with control of both family background and children’s responses at 16 months, mothers’ parenting behavior predicted the changes between children’s responses at 16 to 22 months. The results suggested that maternal repetition of children’s verbal behaviors predicted the change of children’s independent completion between 16 and 22 months of age. On the other hand, children’s behaviors at 16 months predicted children’s neglected and defiant responses at 22 months when eliminating the effects of variables of family background. The results suggested those children’s neglect and refusal behaviors predicted children’s neglected and defiant behaviors at 22 months of old. In conclusion, mothers’ verbal parenting behaviors and mother-toddler joint-attentional behaviors were significantly different between new immigrant mothers and those of local mothers during early childhood. Mothers’ repetition of children’s verbal behaviors promoted the children’s independent completion behaviors.
內政部 (2003)。外籍與大陸配偶生活狀況調查摘要報告。中華民國行政院內政部。
內政部 (2006)。內政統計通報 : 95年底在我國之外籍人士統計。中華民國行政院內政部。
內政部 (2007a)。內政統計通報:95年新生嬰兒生母狀況分析。中華民國行政院內政部。
內政部 (2007b)。內政統計通報:95年各縣市外籍配偶人數與大陸(含港澳)配偶人數。中華民國行政院內政部。
內政部 (2007c)。內政統計通報 : 96年底在我國之外籍人士統計。中華民國行政院內政部。
內政部 (2008)。內政統計通報:97年嬰兒出生狀況統計。中華民國行政院內政部。
內政部 (2009)。 內政統計通報 : 97年底在我國之外籍人士統計。中華民國行政院內政部。
田閔如 (2005)。在台大陸女性配偶生活適應與第二代子女教養狀況之探討--以台東縣為例。未發表之碩士論文,國立東華大學公共行政研究所。
林生傳 (1990)。教育社會學。台北市:巨流圖書公司.
徐澄清 (1977)。因材施教 : 氣質與兒童發展。台北市:健康世界。
莊禮傳 (2001)。外籍新娘之服務及輔導。發表於國際婚姻與外籍新娘座談會,台北市,8月30日。
徐如美 (2004)。外籍母親親職教育課程實施之研究。未發表之碩士論文,國立臺南大學教育經營與管理研究所,臺南市。
陳小紅 (1999)。大陸配偶在台生活狀況案例訪視。台北:行政院大陸委員會。
湯志安 (1994)。母親以負向言語與分離焦慮為管教手段之研究。未發表之碩士論文,文化大學兒童福利學系。
黃世琤與李麗雯 (2008)。母親教養行為中介一至三歲幼兒服從行為發展之研究。發表於第47屆台灣心理學年會。
黃世琤與李麗雯 (出版中)。母親教養行為與幼兒完成作業反應關聯性之追蹤研究。教育心理學報。
黃倩玉 (2005)。新移民女性及其子女發展與適應研究---原生與在地之間:東南亞裔新移民女性的兩個文化(子計畫五研究成果報告):中華民國國家科學委員會,NSC 94-2420-H-007-001-KD。
黃森泉與張雯雁 (2003)。外籍新娘婚姻適應與子女教養問題之探討。社會科教育研究,8,136-169。
劉秀琪 (2004)。苗栗地區台灣、外籍、大陸育齡婦女育嬰知識、育嬰態度、社會支持與嬰兒生長發育狀況之探討。未發表之碩士論文,國立台北護理學院護理學研究所,台北市。
盧秀芳 (2004)。在台外籍新娘子女家庭環境與學校生活適應之研究。未發表之碩士論文,國立政治大學學校行政碩士班。
蕭昭娟 (2000)。國際遷移之調適研究:以彰化縣社頭鄉外籍新娘為例。未發表之碩士論文,國立台灣師範大學地理研究所,台北市。
韓嘉玲 (2003)。傭人抑或太太﹖婦女勞動力的跨境遷移--大陸新娘在台灣案例研究。社區發展季刊,101,163-175。new window
蘇建文、林美珍、陳李綢、程小危、吳敏而、林惠雅、幸曼玲(1996)。發展心理學. 台北:心理出版社。
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1973). The development of infant-mother attachment. In B.M.Caldwell & H.N.Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of Child Development Research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Adair, J. G. (1984). The Hawthorne effect: Are consideration of the methodological artifact. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 334-345.
Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants'' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62(5), 875-890.
Belsky, J., Goode, M. K., & Most, R. K. (1980). Maternal stimulation and infant exploratory competence: Cross-sectional, correlational, and experimental analyses. Child Development, 51(4), 1168-1178.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. London: Hogarth Press.
Brachfeld-Child, S., Simpson, T., & Izenson, N. (1988). Mothers'' and fathers'' speech to infants in a teaching situation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 9(2), 173-180.
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., & Wang, X. Y. (2001). Parental sensitivity, infant affect, and affect regulation: Predictors of later attachment. Child Development, 72(1), 252-270.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723-742.
Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral, and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7(3), 350-369.
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., Liu, M., Chen, H., Wang, L., Li, D., et al. (2003). Compliance in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(5), 428-436.
Crockenberg, S. (1987). Predictors and correlates of anger toward and punitive control of toddlers by adolescent mothers. Child Development, 58(4), 964-975.
Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 961-971.
Dallaire, D. H., & Weinraub, M. (2005). The stability of parenting behaviors over the first 6 years of life. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20(2), 201-219.
Davis, P. W. (1996). Threats of corporal punishment as verbal aggression: A naturalistic study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 20(4), 289-304.
Delin, C., & Baumeister, R. (1994). Praise: More than just social reinforcement. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 24(3), 219-241.
Dix, T., Gershoff, E. T., Meunier, L. N., & Miller, P. C. (2004). The affective structure of supportive parenting: depressive symptoms, immediate emotions, and child-oriented motivation. Dev.Psychol., 40(6), 1212-1227.
Dix, T., Stewart, A. D., Gershoff, E. T., & Day, W. H. (2007). Autonomy and children''s reactions to being controlled: Evidence that both compliance and defiance may be positive markers in early development. Child Development, 78(4), 1204-1221.
Dodici, B. J., Draper, D. C., & Peterson, C. A. (2003). Early parent-child interactions and early literacy development. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 23(3), 124-136.
Driscoll, J. R., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (2007). Young mothers'' play with their toddlers: individual variability as a function of psychosocial factors. Infant & Child Development, 16(6), 649-670.
Erikson, E. H. (1969). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E. H. (1982). The life cycle completed: review. New York: W. W. Norton.
Fiese, B. H. (1990). Playful relationships: A contextual analysis of mother-toddler interaction and symbolic play. Child Development, 61(5), 1648-1656.
Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Fogel, A., Messinger, D. S., Dickson, K. L., & Hsu, H.-c. (1999). Posture and gaze in early mother-infant communication: synchronization of developmental trajectories. Developmental Science, 2(3), 325-332.
Fossett, M. A. (1991). A methodological review of the sex ratio: Alternatives for comparative research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 941-957.
Gable, S., & Isabella, R. (1992). Maternal contributions to infant regulation of arousal. Infant Behavior & Development, 15(1), 95-107.
Garner, P., & Landry, S. (1994). Effects of maternal attention-directing strategies of preterm infants'' affective expressions during joint toy play. Infant Behavior & Development, 17(1), 15-22.
Harter, S., & La Greca, A. M. (1990). Issues in the assessment of the self-concept of children and adolescents Through the eyes of the child: Obtaining self-reports from children and adolescents. (pp. 292-325). Needham Heights, MA, US: Allyn & Bacon.
Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise on children’s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 774-795.
Henderson, E. N., & Jennings, K. D. (2003). Maternal depression and the ability to facilitate joint attention with 18-month-olds. Infancy, 4(1), 27-46.
Hollingshead, A. (1957). Two-factor index of social position New Heaven, Conn.: 1965 Yale Station. Mimeographed.
Hunter, J. E., & Hunter, R. F. (1984). Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job-performance. Psychological Bulletin, 96(1), 72-98.
Hustedt, J. T., & Raver, C. C. (2002). Scaffolding in low-income mother-child dyads: Relations with joint attention and dyadic reciprocity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(2), 113-119.
Kochanska, G., & Kuczynski, L. (1991). Maternal autonomy granting: Predictors of normal and depressed mothers'' compliance with the requests of 5-year-olds. Child Development, 62, 1449-1459.
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64(2), 325-347.
Kochanska, G. (1998). Mother-child relationship, child fearfulness, and emerging attachment: A short-term longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 34(3), May-490.
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72(4), 1091-1111.
Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. T. (2000). Mother-child mutually responsive orientation and conscience development: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 71(2), 417-431.
Kochanska, G., Forman, D. R., Aksan, N., & Dunbar, S. B. (2005). Pathways to conscience: Early mother-child mutually responsive orientation and children''s moral emotion, conduct, and cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 19-34.
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 199-214.
Kuczynski, L., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Development of children''s noncompliance strategies from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 398-408.
Kuczynski, L., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Girnius-Brown, O. (1987). A developmental interpretation of young children''s noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 23(6), 799-806.
Lehman, E. B., Steier, A. J., Guidash, K. M., & Wanna, S. Y. (2002). Predictors of compliancein toddlers: Child temperament, maternal personality and emotional availability. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 301–310.
Linebarger, D. L., & Walker, D. (2005). Infants'' and toddlers'' television viewing and language outcomes. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 624-645.
Maccoby, E. E. (1980). Social development: psychological growth and the parent-child relationship. San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Markus, J., Mundy, P., Morales, M., Delgado, C. E. F., & Yale, M. (2000). Individual differences in infant skills as predictors of child-caregiver joint attention and language. Social Development, 9(3), 302-315.
Mascolo, M. F., Fischer, K. W., Li, J., Richard, J. D., Klaus, R. S., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2003). Dynamic development of component systems of emotions: Pride, shame, and guilt in China and the United States Handbook of Affective Sciences (Vol. 1, pp. 375-408): Oxford University Press.
Matas, L., Arend, R. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547-566.
Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (1998). Give and take: The development of conventional infant gestures. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44(4), 566-590.
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Crowson, M. M., Neal, A. R., & Delgado, C. E. F. (2005). Individual differences in infant attention skills, joint attention, and emotion regulation behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(3), 259-263.
O''Leary, K., & O''Leary, S. (1977). Classroom management. New York: Pergamon Press.
Pinkerton, R., & Scarr, S. (1995). Parental discipline in the United States: Cultural and individual differences. In Policy, punishment and parenting: Parental beliefs and practices in the United States, Sweden and Canada, Scarr, S(Chair). Symposium conducted at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis.
Poulson, C. L., Kyparissos, N., Andreatos, M., Kymissis, E., & Parnes, M. (2002). Generalized imitation within three response classes in typically developing infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81(3), 341-357.
Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E. M., Kuczynski, L., & Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attachment in two- and three-year-olds in normal families and families with parental depression. Child Development, 56(4), 884-893.
Raver, C. C. (1996). Relations between social contingency in mother-child interaction and 2-year-olds'' social competence. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 850.
Rodrigo, M. J., Gonzalez, A., Ato, M., Rodriguez, G., De Vega, M., & Mun eton, M. (2006). Co-development of child-mother gestures over the second and the third years, Infant & Child Development: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Rosen, N. (1989). John and Anzia: An American romance. New York: Dutton.
Rubin, K. H., Stewart, S., Chen, X., & Bornstein, M. H. (1995). Parents of aggressive and withdrawn children Handbook of parenting (pp. 255-284). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Sander, L., & Osofsky, J. D. (1996). Recognition process: Organization and specificity in early development. The handbook of infant development. New York: Wiley.
Shaffer, D. R. (1999). Social and personality development: Wadsworth, Thomson Learning.
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling - its effect on the visual-cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 195-200.
Spitzer, M., Fischbacher, U., Herrnberger, B., Gron, G., & Fehr, E. (2007). The Neural signature of social norm compliance. Neuron, 56(1), 185-196.
Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T., Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A., Greving, K., Liew, J. & Hofer, C. (2007).Relations of Maternal Socialization and Toddlers'' Effortful Control to Children''s Adjustment and Social Competence. Developmental Psychology,. 43(5), 1170-1186.
Sroufe, L. A. (1995). Emotional development: the organization of emotional life in the early years. NY: Cambridge University Press.
Stifter, C. A., Spinrad, T. L., & Braungart-Rieker, J. M. (1999). Toward a developmental model of child compliance: the role of emotion regulation in infancy. Child Development, 70 (1), 21-32.
Stifter, C. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2002). The effect of excessive crying on the development of emotion regulation. Infancy, 3(2), 133-152.
Swanson, K., Beckwith, L., & Howard, J. (2000). Intrusive caregiving and quality of attachment in prenatally drug- exposed toddlers and their primary caregivers. Attachment & Human Development, 2(2), 130-148.
Thelen, E. (1989). Self-organization in developmental processes: Can systems approaches. In M. R. Gunnar & E. Thelen (Eds.), Systems and development, Minnesota Symposia in Child Psychology (pp. 77-117). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4(12), 197.
van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Goldberg, S., Kroonenberg, P. M., & Frenkel, O. J. (1992). The relative effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment: a meta-analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Development, 63(4), 840-858.
Vaughn, B. E., Kopp, C. B., & Krakow, J. B. (1984). The emergence and consolidation of self-control from eighteen to thirty months of age: Normative trends and individual differences. Child Development, 55, 990-1004.
Veneziano, E., Sinclair, H., & Berthoud, I. (1990). From one word to two words: Repetition patterns on the way to structured speech. Journal of Child Language, 17(3), 633-650.
Volling, B. L., Mcelwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C., & Herrera, C. (2002). Parents'' emotional availability and infant emotional competence: Predictors of parent-infant attachment and emerging self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 16(4), 447-465.
Wang, L., Chen, X., Chen, H., Cui, L., & Li, M. (2006). Affect and maternal parenting as predictors of adaptive and maladaptive behaviors in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(2), 158-166.
Wang, Y. Z., Wiley, A. R., & Chiu, C. Y. (2008). Independence-supportive praise versus interdependence-promoting praise. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(1), 13-20.
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top