:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:探索中文語意饜足現象的性質
作者:許樂山 引用關係
作者(外文):BrunoGalmar
校院名稱:國立成功大學
系所名稱:教育研究所
指導教授:陳振宇
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2012
主題關鍵詞:漢字文詞彙辨識語意饜足義遺忘意義提取Verbal SatiationSemantic SatiationLoci of Verbal SatiationSemantic MemoryRepetitive Semantic Processing
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(1) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:1
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:50
重複的唸一個字還是重複的讀一個字有可能會抑制(impair)意義提取 (meaning retrieval)引發時間很短的義遺忘(loss of meaning)。在西方文獻這個現象稱為「語意饜足」(semantic satiation)。Smith and Klein(1990)藉由一個採取類別比對作業(category matching task)發現語意饜足的本性是語意的。根據漢字文詞彙辨識的研究文發現,長時間注視一個字會引發主觀感受的「文字解體」(orthographic decomposition/satiation):受試者漸漸懷疑字組成的現象。(Chen & Wu, 1994)強調文字解體主要是字形的解體現象不是語意的現象。(Chen & Wu, 1994)假定在漢字詞彙辨識沒有語意饜足的現象只有文字解體。 故本研究藉由一個採取類別比對連續(continuous)作業試圖探討中文複合字(Chinese compound words)的類別語意饜足現象(categorical semantic satiation)。
實驗一使用重複的一個類別詞(category word)引發語意饜足。受試者一題又一題判斷一個正例(exemplar)是否屬於一個類別詞。實驗2a使用同一個類別的不同的正例詞引發語意饜足。受試者判斷兩個正例是否屬於同一個類別。實驗2b重複檢驗實驗2a語意饜足的發現。實驗3使用一個word-form判斷作業和同一個類別詞引發語意饜足。受試者判斷兩個類別詞是不是同一個詞。綜合四個實驗之結果,研究者推論漢字的語意饜足現象是一個真正的語意歷程不是字形的解體現象。
實驗4使用英文成功地重複檢驗實驗2a語意饜足的發現。實驗5使用中英文成功地重複檢驗實驗2a語意饜足的發現。實驗5證明cross-language語意饜足的存在。本研究的最後部分描述一些意饜足的的認知語模擬。本研究的結果強調重複的處理類別詞的類別語意引發語意饜足。
The subjective reports collected by experimental psychologists for almost one century (Severance & Washburn, 1907; Moulin, 2006) mention a lapse or a loss of the meaning of a word following its massive repetition either as its prolonged viewing - e.g. 1 min - or its active repetition - (oral or written) - for 30 times. This experienced loss of meaning has been coined semantic satiation (Jakobovits, 1962). The term semantic satiation emphasizes that the locus of satiation is thought to be semantic. However, the existence of this semantic locus remains unproven experimentally. In the present work, we use the term verbal satiation instead of semantic satiation until the locus of satiation has been clearly identified. Verbal satiation is different from the verbal transformation effect (Warren, 1974) because it can be induced simply through passive visual exposure of a repeatedly viewed word without stimulation of the auditory modality. Verbal satiation elicited by the repetitive viewing of a word is not a mere habituation of visual neuronal units: in verbal satiation, the form of a word does not necessarily disappear from sight but its meaning does disappear from awareness.
We focus on verbal satiation of categorical meaning. Firstly, verbal satiation of Chinese disyllabic words is studied in three experiments to ascertain the phenomenon, to track its time course, and to identify its locus. We adapt for Chinese the speeded matching-category task described in (Tian & Huber, 2010) by mainly doubling the size of the experimental block. Experiment 1 asked the participants to judge if an exemplar word matched a category word in 22 blocks of 40 trials each. Within a block, one category word appeared 20 times (repeated trials) while each word of the remaining 10 categories appeared only twice (baseline trials). For the first six trials, response times (RTs) for the repeated trials were similar to RTs for the baseline trials. For the subsequent trials, repeated RTs were slower (by 12 ms) than baseline RTs, indicating a satiation effect. Its loci could be orthographic, semantic, or both, or on the associative links between form and meaning. In Experiment 2a, participants judged if two exemplar words belonged to the same category, but the category word was not shown. Repeated RTs were faster (by 6 ms) than baseline RTs for the first 12 trials. Then, verbal satiation emerged and was of similar magnitude (13 ms) as that observed in Experiment 1. Experiment 2b replicated Experiment 2a successfully. The satiation effect must be semantic, as only categorical meanings were repeated. Experiment 3 asked participants to judge if two category words were identical, mostly an orthographic task. Repeated RTs were faster than baseline RTs across trials, suggesting no orthographic satiation. The results indicate that verbal satiation of Chinese words can be directly semantic (categorical). Its time course conforms to the habituation model of Rankin, et al. (2009) as well as to the neuro-computational simulations of Gotts (2003), i.e., sensitization (semantic priming) followed by habituation (semantic satiation).
Experiments 4 and 5 aimed at replicating the previous results of Experiment 2a respectively for English and across English and Chinese. In Experiment 4, verbal satiation is demonstrated in English with native English speakers, demonstrating the language independent nature of verbal satiation. Experiment 5 showed that cross-language satiation is possible between English and Chinese for Taiwanese participants. The time course of bilingual verbal satiation is similar to the one of monolingual verbal satiation. In the last part of the thesis, we modeled and simulated simply the time course of verbal satiation in Experiment 2a using as a core concept a recurrent neuron unit with excitatory feedback connection. The model acts with a memory of the eight previous exemplars to operate novelty and redundancy detection and to induce sensitization or habituation of the response of the repeated categorical meaning neuron unit. This model accounted for the reversal of semantic priming into semantic satiation, and even captured the total recovery from semantic satiation at the end of the block. We concluded by exposing implications of the work for broader theories of cognition and by listing future research directions. We also finally and tentatively forecast the future of verbal satiation as a promising experimental paradigm.
Abbott, L., Varela, J., Sen, K., & Nelson, S. (1997). Synaptic depression and cortical gain control. Science, 275(5297), 221.
Anastasio T J. Tutorial on Neural Systems Modeling. Sunderland (MA): Sinauer Associates Inc.; 2010.
Anderson, J. R. (2002). Spanning seven orders of magnitude: A challenge for cognitive modeling. Cognitive science, 26(1), 85-112.
Anderson, M. C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanisms of forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 415-445.
Baars, B. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge Univ Pr.
Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind: Oxford University Press, USA.
Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behavior Research Methods, 37(3), 379-384.
Balota, D., Yap, M., & Cortese, M. (2006). Visual word recognition: The journey from features to meaning (a travel update). Handbook of psycholinguistics, 285–375.
Bäuml, K. H. (2002). Semantic generation can cause episodic forgetting. Psychological Science, 13(4), 356-360.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 59, 617-645.
Black, S. R. (2003). Review of semantic satiation. Advances in Psychology Research, 63.
Blaxton, T. A., & Neely, J. H. (1983). Inhibition from semantically related primes: Evidence of a category-specific inhibition. Memory & cognition, 11(5), 500-510.
Bransford, J. (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school: National Academies Press.
Brown AS, Zoccoli SL, Leahy MM. Cumulating Retrieval Inhibition in Semantic and Lexical Domains. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition.2005;31(3):496–50712.
Chen, K. J., Huang, C. R., Chang, L. P., & Hsu, H. L. (1996). Sinica corpus: Design methodology for balanced corpora. Language, 167, 176.
Cheng, C. M., & Lan, Y. H. (2009). An implicit test of Chinese orthographic satiation. Reading and Writing, 1-36.new window
Cheng, C. M., & Wu, S. J. (1994). Orthographic satiation and disorganization in Chinese. Advances in the study of Chinese language processing, 1, 1-30.
Chen, J., & Su, J. (2009). Morphological Satiation in Continuous Lexical Decisions of Chinese Compound Words. Unpublished report.
Cieslicka, A. (2004). Semantic priming effects in bilingualword fragment completion. Poznan Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 40, 111–133.
Collins, A., & Loftus, E. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological review, 82(6), 407–428.
Cowan, N. (2001) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 87–185
Deacon, D., Dynowska, A., Ritter, W., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2004). Repetition and semantic priming of nonwords: Implications for theories of N400 and word recognition. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 60–74.
Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - USA, 93, 13494-13499.
Dodson, C. S., Johnson, M. K., & Schooler, J. W. (1997). The verbal overshadowing effect: Why descriptions impair face recognition. Memory & cognition, 25(2), 129-139.
Douglas RJ, Koch C, Mahowald M, Martin KA, Suarez HH (1995) Recurrent excitation in neocortical circuits. Science 269: 981-985.
Esposito, N., & Pelton, L. (1971). Review of the measurement of semantic satiation.
Psychological Bulletin, 75(5), 330–346.
Farah, M. J. (1995). Visual agnosia: Disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision: The MIT Press Cambridge, MA.
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, 35(1), 116-124.
Francis, W. S. (1999). Cognitive integration of language and memory in bilinguals: Semantic representation. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 193.
Franklin, S., Baars, B., Ramamurthy, U., & Ventura, M. (2005). The role of consciousness in memory. Brains, Minds and Media, 1(1), 38.
Frenck-Mestre, C., Besson, M., & Pynte, J. (1997). Finding the locus of semantic satiation: An electrophysiological attempt. Brain and language, 57(3), 406-422.
Gotts, S. J. (2003). Mechanisms underlying enhanced processing efficiency in neural systems. PhD Thesis, Carnegie Mellon University.
Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A. (2006) Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 14–23.
Herculano-Houzel S (2009) The human brain in numbers: A linearly scaled-up primate brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
Hutchison, K. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 785.
Izhikevich, E. M., & Edelman, G. M. (2008). Large-scale model of mammalian thalamocortical systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(9), 3593-3598.
Jakobovits, L. (1962). Effects of repeated stimulation on cognitive aspects of behavior: Some experiments on the phenomenon of semantic satiation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, McGill University.
Jakobovits, L., & Lambert, W. (1961). Semantic satiation among bilinguals. Journal of experimental psychology, 62(6), 576–582.
Jeng, C.I., Lai, M.W.; Liu, I.M. (1973). Category norms in Chinese and English from bilingual participants. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 15, 81-153.
Johnson, S. K., & Anderson, M. C. (2004). The role of inhibitory control in forgetting semantic knowledge. Psychological Science, 15(7), 448-453.
Kornmeier, J., & Bach, M. (2005). The necker cube–an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing. Vision Research, 45(8), 955–960.
Kounios, J., Kotz, S. A., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). On the locus of the semantic satiation effect: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Memory and Cognition, 28(8), 1366-1377.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual review of psychology, 62, 621.
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological review, 104(2), 211.
Laws, D. (1995). Verbal satiation: Notes on procedure, with speculations on its mechanism
of effect. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 7(2), 155.
Lee, N. C. (2007). Perceptual Coherence of Chinese Characters: Orthographic Satiation and Disorganization. Master thesis, The University of Edinburgh.
Lewis, M. B., & Ellis, H. D. (2000). Satiation in name and face recognition. Memory & cognition, 28(5), 783.
Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: a meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 618.
Macrae, C. N., & MacLeod, M. D. (1999). On recollections lost: When practice makes imperfect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 463–473.
Markram H (2006) The blue brain project. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:153–160.
Markram, H., & Tsodyks, M. (1996). Redistribution of synaptic efficacy between neocortical pyramidal neurons. NATURE, 382, 807.
McClelland, J. L. (2009). The place of modeling in cognitive science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(1), 11-38.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological review, 63(2), 81.
Minzenberg, M. J., Ober, B. A., & Vinogradov, S. (2002). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: a review and synthesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(05), 699-720.
Moulin, C. J., & Connor, A. R. (2006). Semantic satiation and subjective experience: The strange case of jamais vu. Paper presented at the International Conference of Memory, Sydney.
Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological review, 76(2), 165–178.
Neely, J. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theory. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.) Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264-336). Hillsadale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Neely, J. H., VerWys, C. A., & Kahan, T. A. (1998). Reading Glasses will prime vision, but reading a pair of glasses will not. Memory & cognition, 26(1), 34-39.
Ornstein, R. (1996). The psychology of consciousness. Penguin Books New York.
Pomarol-Clotet, E., Oh, T., Laws, K., & McKenna, P. (2008). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 192(2), 92-97.
Pritchard, R., Heron,W., & Hebb, D. (1960). Visual perception approached by the method of stabilized images.
Prochwicz, K. (2011). Empty words in schizophrenia. Semantic satiation as the mechanism of formal language disorders. European Psychiatry, 26, 1483-1483.
Pynte, J. (1991). The locus of semantic satiation in category membership decision and acceptability judgment. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20(4), 315–335.
Rankin, C. H., Abrams, T., Barry, R. J., Bhatnagar, S., Clayton, D. F., Colombo, J., et al. (2009). Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 92(2), 135-138.
Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid* 1. Cognitive Psychology, 22(1), 36-71.
Severance, E., & Washburn, M. F. (1907). The loss of associative power in words after long fixation. The American Journal of Psychology, 18(2), 182-186.
Shimokido, T. (2007). An effect of semantic satiation in conceptual processing. Shinrigaku Kenkyu, 78(5), 469–477.
Silver, R., Boahen, K., Grillner, S., Kopell, N., & Olsen, K. L. (2007). Neurotech for Neuroscience: Unifying Concepts, Organizing Principles, and Emerging Tools. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(44), 11807-11819.
Smith, L. (1984). Semantic satiation affects category membership decision time but not lexical priming. Memory & Cognition, 12(5), 483–488.
Smith, L., & Klein, R. (1990). Evidence for semantic satiation: Repeating a category slows subsequent semantic processing. Learning, Memory, 16(5), 852-861.
Tai, James H-Y. 1994. Chinese classifier systems and human categorization. In Honor of
Professor William S-Y. Wang: Interdisciplinary Studies on Language and Language Change, ed. by Matthew Chen and Ovid Tseng, 479-494. Taipei: Pyramid Publishing Company
Tian, X. (2008). Testing a dynamic account of neural processing: behavioral and electrophsyiological studies of semantic satiation. PhD Thesis.
Tian, X., & Huber, D. E. (2010). Testing an associative account of semantic satiation. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 267-290.
Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(2), 321-343.
Toni, I., De Lange, F., Noordzij, M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). Language beyond action. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1-3), 71–79.
Tsodyks, M., & Markram, H. (1997). The neural code between neocortical pyramidal neurons depends on neurotransmitter release probability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94(2), 719.
Van Overschelde, J. P., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the norms. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(3), 289-335.
Warren, M. R. (1961) Illusory changes of distinct speech upon repetition – the verbal transformation effect. British Journal of Psychology 52:249–58.
Warren, R. (1974). Verbal transformation effect and auditory perceptual mechanisms. Speech and hearing science: selected readings, 70(4), 124.
Weger, U. W., & Inhoff, A. W. (2006). Semantic inhibition of return is the exception rather than the rule. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 68(2), 244-253.
Wickens DD. Some characteristics of word encoding. Memory & Cognition. 1973;1:485–490.

 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top