:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:兒童對同儕言語挑釁之受傷與易怒敏感度:社會計量差異與發展差異
書刊名:中華心理學刊
作者:葉北辰雷庚玲 引用關係
作者(外文):Yeh, Pei-chenLay, Keng-ling
出版日期:2008
卷期:50:1
頁次:頁49-69
主題關鍵詞:先機式攻擊回應式攻擊同儕關係受傷敏感度易怒敏感度Proactive aggressionReactive aggressionPeer relationshipVulnerabilityInflammability
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(3) 博士論文(2) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:3
  • 共同引用共同引用:13
  • 點閱點閱:230
負向言語挑釁是國小學童間非常有效且常見的攻擊手段。本研究的目的為探討國小學童面對不同喜歡程度的同儕對其施以負向言語挑釁時,在受傷敏感度和易怒敏感度上的發展趨勢。性別差異;以及分別探究在先機式攻擊。回應式攻擊和被欺負三種不適應的同儕互動向度上分數高和低的國小學童對同儕施以負向言語的反應差異。總共有720位四年級與六年級國小學童接受施測。第一階段施測以社會提名法,請受試者選出班上最喜歡。最不喜歡。具先機式攻擊。回應式攻擊。及被欺負傾向的同學。第二階段則偽裝為另一實驗,以第一階段所提名的最喜歡與最不喜歡的同學為個人實驗刺激,利用假想故事情境,測量當不同施話者(喜歡或不喜歡的同學)說出不同言語刺激(中性或負向)時,國小學童的易怒敏感度(包括生氣與敵意歸因)與受傷敏感度(包括悲傷與自責歸因)。以全體受試者針對兩個依變項所分別進行之兩個「年級(2)×性別(2)×施話者(2)×言語內容(2)」之四因子變異數分析呈現,受傷敏感度和易怒敏感度會隨著年齡降低;兒童面對喜歡的人說負向言語受傷敏感度較高、面對不喜歡的人說負向言語則是易怒敏感度較高。而三個分別以先機式攻擊。回應式攻擊和被欺負分數高/低來分組的「組別(2)×施話者(2)×言語內容(2)」三因子變異數分析發現:先機式攻擊傾向高的學童受傷敏感度較對照組低,被欺負傾向高的學童受傷敏感度較對照組高;但是回應式攻擊傾向高的學童易怒敏感度並沒有如預期較對照組高。
When children enter middle childhood, mode of aggression becomes increasingly less physical. Instead, verbal aggression occurs frequently in peer interaction of elementary school students. The purpose of this research is to investigate, during middle and late childhood, the age, sex, and individual differences of the vulnerability and inflammability in response to peer verbal aggression delivered either by liked or disliked classmates. Vulnerability is measured by children's fear and self-blame in response to peer provocation. Inflammability is measured by children's anger and hostility attributions. Whitesell and Harter (1996) indicated that children respond with more intense and prolonged negative emotion if conflict situations involve best friends. Fabes, Eisenberg, Smith, and Murphy (1996), by contrast, demonstrated that children's responses to provocations by well liked peers are more controlled than is the case for anger provoked by peers who are not well liked. The first goal of this study thus is to compare children's response to verbal provocation enacted by liked versus disliked classmates. Selman and Byrne (1974) suggested that, from middle to late childhood, children move from being incapable of integrating self and other's perspectives to simultaneously considering mutual roles during interactions. Crick, Grotpeter, and Bigbee (2002) as well as Underwood, Coie, and Herbsman (1992) and Underwood, Hurley, Johanson, and Mosley (1999) suggested that older children are better at regulating their anger and distress. That is, older children are better both at understanding other's perspective and regulating one's own emotion. The second goal of this study thus is to test the hypothesis of milder vulnerability and inflammability for older children in response to peer verbal aggression. Previous research on the Social Information Processing (SIP) of proactive aggressors, reactive aggressors, and victimized children suggested that each type of children may react to peer conflict with different encoding, interpretation, response evaluation, and response enactment. Accordingly, the third goal of this study is to examine whether children with high reactive aggression tendency are more likely to be inflamed by both negative and neutral peer verbal provocation (Dodge & Coie, 1987; Crick & Dodge, 1996); whether those with high proactive aggression tendency, because of their emotional-callous traits (Frick et al., 2003; Kimonis et al., 2006), are less vulnerable and inflammable to verbal provocation; and whether those with high victimization tendency are more vulnerable and less inflammable while facing peer verbal provocation (Finnegan, Hodges, & Perry, 1998; Schwartz, Chang, & Farver, 2001). Eleven classes of fourth graders (189 males and 166 females) and eleven classes of sixth graders (190 males and 175 females) from the Metropolitan Taipei area participated in the two-stage study. During the first stage of data collection, information about each subject's liked and disliked classmates and the proactive aggressors, reactive aggressors, and victims in the class was collected through sociometrics. Results revealed that those who scored lower on social preference and those who scored higher on social impact were more likely to be nominated as proactive aggressors, reactive aggressors, or victims of aggression (all ps<.01). During the second stage of data collection, each subject read hypothetical scenarios about receiving verbal aggressions and neutral verbal stimuli from the particular liked or disliked classmates that the child had nominated during the first stage. Subjects were to rate their fear, self-blame, anger, and hostility attribution in response to the scenarios. Concordant with Whitesell and Harter (1996), results demonstrated that fourth graders revealed higher vulnerability in response to negative verbal provocation delivered by liked as opposed to disliked peers (effect of "liked vs. disliked peer x age" interaction on vulnerability: F(1, 499)=10.84, p<.01). Contrary to Whitesell and Harter (1996), but concordant with Fabes et al. (1996), verbal aggression from disliked peers resulted in higher inflammability, F(1, 499)=371.57, p<.001. Sixth graders were less inflammable to verbal aggression than their fourth grade counterparts, F(1, 499)=10.02, p<.01. No sex differences were found in sixth graders' inflammability, but fourth grade boys' inflammability was significantly higher than girls, F(1, 499)=6.90, p<.01. The above results indicate the development of children's emotional regulation and social information processing from the fourth to the sixth grade especially in boys. Children with high proactive-aggression tendency experienced lower vulnerability, F(1, 162)=14.89, p<.01, and children with high victimization tendency experienced higher vulnerability F(1, 166)=8.11, p<.01. upon being verbally provoked by peers, In addition, children with high victimization tendency responded to liked versus disliked peers' negative verbal provocation with less differentiated level of inflammability as compared with the non-victimized children (the effect of "liked vs. disliked peers x victimization" interaction on inflammability : F(1, 166)=14.61, p<.01). Contrary to the original hypothesis, children with high reactive-aggression tendency did not show higher inflammability in response to peer verbal aggression. The methodological and theoretical implications of the present findings were discussed.
期刊論文
1.Craig, W. M.(1998)。The Relationship among Bullying, Victimization, Depression, Anxiety, and Aggression in Elementary School Children。Personality and Individual Difference,24,123-130。  new window
2.Zakriski, A. L.、Coie, J. D.(1996)。A Comparison of Aggressive-rejected and Nonaggressive-rejected Children's Interpretations of Self-directed and Other-directed Rejection。Child Development,67,1048-1070。  new window
3.Dodge, K. A.、Lochman, J. E.、Harnish, J. D.、Bates, J. E.、Pettit, G. S.(1997)。Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth。Journal of Abnormal Psychology,106(1),37-51。  new window
4.Crick, N. R.、Dodge, K. A.(1994)。A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment。Psychological Bulletin,115(1),74-101。  new window
5.洪儷瑜、涂春仁(19960100)。Coie & Dodge社會計量地位分類公式之修正。測驗年刊,43,103-114。new window  延伸查詢new window
6.Dodge, Kenneth A.、Frame, Cynthia L.(1982)。Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys。Child Development,53(3),620-635。  new window
7.Graham, S.、Jovonen, J.(1998)。Self-blame and peer victimization in middle school: An attributional analysis。Developmental Psychology,34(3),587-599。  new window
8.Selman, R. L.、Byrne, D. F. A.(1974)。A structural-developmental analysis of levels of role-taking in middle childhood。Child Development,45(3),803-806。  new window
9.Dodge, K. A.(1980)。Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior。Child Development,51(1),162-170。  new window
10.Borg, M. G.(1998)。The emotional reactions of school bullies and their victims。Educational Psychology,18(4),433-445。  new window
11.Coie, J. D.、Coppotelli, H.、Dodge, K. A.(1982)。Dimensions and types of social status: A cross-age perspective。Developmental Psychology,18(4),557-570。  new window
12.Dodge, K. A.、Crick, N. R.(1996)。Social Information-processing Mechanisms in Reactive and Proactive Aggression。Child Development,67(3),993-1002。  new window
13.Dodge, Kenneth A.、Coie, John D.(1987)。Social-Information-Processing Factors in Reactive and Proactive Aggression in Children's Peer Groups。Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,53(6),1146-1158。  new window
14.Hartup, W. W.、van Lieshout, C. F. M.、van Ijzendoorn, H. W.、Cillessen, A. H. N.(1992)。Heterogeneity among Peer-rejected Boys: Subtypes and Stabilities。Child Development,63,893-905。  new window
15.Björkqvist, K.(1994)。Sex Differences in Physical, Verbal, and Indirect Aggression: A Review of Recent Research。Sex Roles,30,177-188。  new window
16.Poulin, F.、Boivin, M.(2000)。The Role of Proactive and Reactive Aggression in the Formation and Development of Boys' Friendships。Developmental Psychology,36,233-240。  new window
17.Underwood, M. K.、Hurley, J. C.、Schockner, A. E.(2001)。Children's Responses to Same- and other-gender Peers: An Experimental Investigation with 8-, 10-, and 12-year-olds。Developmental Psychology,37,362-372。  new window
18.Farver, J. M.、Chang, L.、Schwartz, D.(2001)。Correlates of Victimization in Chinese Children's Peer Groups。Developmental Psychology,37,520-532。  new window
19.Dane, H. E.、Loney, B. R.、Barry, C. T.、Cornell, A. H.、Bodin, S. D.、Frick, P. J.(2003)。Callousunemotional Traits and Developmental Pathways to Severe Conduct Problems。Developmental Psychology,39,246-260。  new window
20.Dodge, K. A.、McClaskey, C. L.、Brown, M. M.、Pettit, G. S.(1986)。Social Competence in Children。Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,51,1-85。  new window
21.Dodge, K. A.、Pettit, G. S.、Coie, J. D.、Price, J. M.(1990)。Peer Status and Aggression in Boys' Groups: Development and Contextual Analyses。Child Development,61,1289-1309。  new window
22.Terry, R.、Wright, V.、Coie, J. D.、Dodge, K. A.(1991)。The Role of Aggression in Peer Relations: An Analysis of Aggression Episodes in Boys' Play Groups。Child Development,62,812-826。  new window
23.Coie, J. D.、Herbsman, C. R.、Underwood, M. K.(1992)。Display Rules of Anger and Aggression in School-age Children。Child Development,63,366-380。  new window
24.Price, J. M.、Dodge, K. A.(1994)。On the Relation between Social Information Processing and Socially Competent Behavior in Early School-aged Children。Child Development,65,1383-1397。  new window
25.Murphy, B. C.、Fabes, R. A.、Smith, M. C.、Eisenberg, N.(1996)。Getting Angry at Peers: Associations with Liking of the Provocateur。Child Development,67,942-956。  new window
26.Harter, S.、Whitesell, N. R.(1996)。The Interpersonal Context of Emotion: Anger with Close Friends and Classmates。Child Development,67(4),1345-1359。  new window
27.Hurley, J. C.、Johanson, C. A.、Underwood, M. K.、Mos l ey, J. E.(1999)。An Experimental, Observational Investigation of Children's Responses to Peer Provocation: Developmental and Gender Differences in Middle Childhood。Child Development,70,1428-1446。  new window
28.Bigbee, M. A.、Grotpeter, J. K.、Crick, N. R.(2002)。Relationally and Physically Aggressive Children's Intent Attributions and Feelings of Distress for Relational and Instrumental Peer Provocations。Child Development,73,1134-1142。  new window
29.Hodges, E. V. E.、Perry, D. G.、Finnegan, R. A.(1998)。Victimization by Peer: Associations with Children's Reports of Mother-child Interaction。Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75,1076-1086。  new window
30.Bartini, M.、Brooks, F.、Pellegrini, A. D.(1999)。School Bullies, Victims, and Aggressive Victims: Factors Relating to Group Affiliation and Victimization in Early Adolescence。Journal of Educational Psychology,91,216-224。  new window
31.Farrell, J. M.、Cornell, A. H.、Smyke, A. T.、Frick, P. J.、Kimonis, E. R.、Boris, N. W.(2006)。Callousunemotional Features, Behavioral Inhibition, and Parenting: Independent Predictors of Aggression in a High-risk Preschool Sample。Journal of Child and Family Studies,15,745-756。  new window
32.Mahady, W.、Melissa, M.、Pepler, D. J.、Craig, W. M.(2000)。Emotional Regulation and Display in Classroom Victims of Bullying: Characteristic Expressions of Affect, Coping Styles and Relevant Contextual Factors。Social Development,9,226-245。  new window
33.Shipman, K.、Zeman, J.(1996)。Children's Expression of Negative Affect: Reasons and Methods。Developmental Psychology,32,842-849。  new window
會議論文
1.Lay, K.-L.、Lee, M.-C.(1999)。The Relation between First-graders' Verbal Aggression Tendency toward Peers and Their Vulnerability to Hypothetical Maternal Verbal Abuse。Albuquerque, NM。  new window
圖書
1.Selman, R. L.(1980)。The Growth of Interpersonal Understanding。New York, NY:Academic Press。  new window
2.Gottman, J. M.(1986)。The Observation of Social Process。Conversations of Friends: Speculations on Affective Development。New York, NY。  new window
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
:::
QR Code
QRCODE