:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:中文學齡前幼童的非詞覆誦、詞彙量及音韻能力在發展過程中之動態互動:跨序列研究
作者:李乃欣
作者(外文):Naihsin Li
校院名稱:臺灣大學
系所名稱:語言學研究所
指導教授:張顯達
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2015
主題關鍵詞:非詞覆誦接受性詞彙表達性詞彙音韻口語輸出能力nonword repetitionreceptive vocabularyexpressive vocabularyproductive phonology
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(0) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:36
本研究的目的在於探討非詞覆誦及語言知識(即辭彙量及音韻能力)在發展過程中之動態互動。首先,本研究提出一個工作模型,並預測隨著幼童詞彙量的增加,音韻能力及詞彙量會對於非詞覆誦表現有不同比重的貢獻及影響。此外,本研究也檢視非詞覆誦對於表達性詞彙發展的預測能力。此研究採用了跨序列實驗設計,追蹤三個不同年齡組的小孩(即兩歲、三歲及四歲)各一年的時間。小孩每半年接受一次測試,共受測三次。測試內容包含理解性詞彙,表達性詞彙,音韻口語輸出能力,詞彙區辨,及非詞覆誦。非詞覆誦包含了兩項次作業,分別為暫時詞覆誦(由中文現存音節所組合之非詞)及空缺詞覆誦(由中文之空缺音節所組合之非詞)。研究結果顯示幼童在非詞覆誦的表現隨年紀增長而進步,然而同齡孩童之間存在顯著的個體間差異。分析指出其個體間差異來自孩童的詞彙知識及音韻口語輸出能力之影響,而它們影響的程度則取決於詞彙量及非詞刺激材料的特性。隨著孩童詞彙量的增加,詞彙知識會支持並增進他們在非詞覆誦之表現。音韻口語輸出能力也會調節孩童在非詞覆誦的表現,但其效果發生在詞彙效應之後。當孩童達到相當的詞彙量,並皆會以詞彙資源來處理非詞時,他們的個體間差異則轉而取決於他們的音韻口語輸出能力。暫時詞覆誦作業及空缺詞覆誦作業在非詞刺激材料上的差異,則可反映不同語言層面在新詞處理時的作用。當兩者都受到音韻口語輸出能力的影響時,暫時詞覆誦會額外受到詞彙知識的影響。基於這些發現,本研究最後提出了一個修正模型來說明非詞覆誦的機制。同時本研究也證明孩童在非詞覆誦的表現可預測他們在表達性詞彙的發展。
This study explored the dynamic relationship among language knowledge (i.e. vocabulary size and phonological capacities) and nonword repetition (NWR). Specifically, we proposed a three-phase working model which predicted that phonological capacities and vocabulary size might be dominating factors to NWR at different phases. Moreover, we examined the predictability of NWR to expressive vocabulary development. The study was conducted in a cross-sequential design. We recruited three cohorts of typically-developing children, respectively from the ages of 2, 3, and 4. They were followed for one year, tested at three time points, with an interval of 6 months. The children were tested with receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, productive phonology, word discrimination, and NWR, which included nonce word repetition and gap word repetition. Inspection on children’s NWR revealed growth with age. However, children of the same age manifested considerable individual variation. Findings of the analyses verified that productive phonology and vocabulary knowledge played roles in children’s NWR development. Nevertheless, the extent to which they contributed to the variance in NWR was determined by the increase in vocabulary size and the nature of the stimuli. The effect of lexical knowledge was consistently found in children from age 2 to age 5, as evidenced by the vocabulary breadth effect in the age 2 and the lexicality effect in the older children. The findings indicated that children made use of existing vocabulary knowledge to support their encoding of novel sound forms. The mediation of productive phonology to NWR usually occurred after the mediation of vocabulary; however, productive phonology took over the role of determining NWR variation when children reached a certain level of vocabulary size and learned to retrieve for lexical support when encoding nonwords. The repetition of nonce words and the repetition of gap words were found to involved different processes. While the repetition of both types of nonwords was mediated by productive phonology, the repetition of nonce words was additionally supported by lexical knowledge. Based on the finding above, a revised model was developed to account for the processes involved in NWR. Furthermore, our study provided the evidence that NWR could predict children’s subsequent expressive vocabulary knowledge.
Abel, A. D, & Schuele, M. (2014). The influence of two cognitive-linguistic variables on incidental word learning in 5-year-olds. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43, 477-463.
Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S.E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 403-414.
Adams, A-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1996). Phonological working memory and spoken language development in young children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 216-233.
Anthony, J. L., Williams, J. M., Aghara, R. G., Dunkelberger, M., Novak, B., & Mukherjee, A. D. (2010). Assessment of individual differences in phonological representation. Reading and Writing, 23, 969-994.
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 970-983.
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition and serial recall: Equivalent measures of verbal short-term memory? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 587-606.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Short-term phonological memory and long-term learning: A single case study. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 129-148.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158-173.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press.
Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Couteur, A. (1998). Autism: The phenotype in relatives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 369-389.
Barrouillet, P., Gavens, N., Vergauwe, E., Gaillard, V., & Camos, V. (2009). Working memory span development: A time-based resource-sharing model account. Developmental Psychology, 45, 477-490.
Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camons, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Genera, 133, 83-100.
Bell, R. Q. (1953). Convergence: An accelerated longitudinal approach. Child Development, 24, 145-152.
Best, C. T. (1984). Discovering messages in the medium: Speech and the prelinguistic infant. In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. Lester, & M. Yogman (Eds.), Advances in pediatric psychology (pp. 97-145). New York: Plenum.
Bishop, D. V., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioral marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 37, 391-403.
Bowey, J. A. (1996). On the association between phonological memory and receptive vocabulary in five-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 44-78.
Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children’s receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441-469.
Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386-404.
Chang, Cheng-feng, & Chung, Yu-Mei. (1986). The Chinese version of Preschool Language Scale and its related studies. Bulletin of Special Education, 2, 37-52.
Chen, Li-mei. (2005). Development of vocalic production in a Mandarin-learning infant: A longitudinal case study. Providence Forum: Language and Humanities, 1, 169-192.
Chen, W. (2011). Comparing the phonological and articulation abilities between stuttering and nonstuttering Mandarin-speaking children (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Pingtung University, Pingtung, Taiwan.
Chen, Y. (2012). Novel word learning and phonological processing abilities in preschool children with specific language impairment (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan.
Cheung, H. (1998). The application of MLU in Chinese. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Association of ROC, 13, 36-48.
Cheung, H. (2000). Three to four-year-old children’s perception and production of Mandarin consonants. Language and Linguistics, 1, 19-38.
Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The preschool repetition test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 429-443.new window
Cho, Shih-Chieh. (2008). The phonological development of 3 to 6 year-old preschool children in Taiwan (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Taipei College of Nursing, Taipei, Taiwan.
Clark, E. V. (2003). First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Coady, J. A, & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 1-40.
Coady, J, Evans, J. L., & Kluender, K. R. (2010). Role of phonotactic frequency in nonword repetition by children with specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 4, 494-509.
Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1029-1037.
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62-101). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
D’odorico, L., Assanelli, A., Franco, F., & Jacob, V. (2007). A follow-up study on Italian late talkers: Development of language, short-term memory, phonological awareness, impulsiveness, and attention. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 157-169.
Dollaghan, C. A., Biber, M. E., & Campbell, T. F. (1995). Lexical influences on nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 211-222.
Dollaghan, C. A., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1136-1146.
Edwards, J., Beckman, M. E., & Munson, B. (2004). The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children’s production accuracy and fluency in novel word repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 421-436.
Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1998). Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment: Exploration of some explanations for their inaccuracies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 279-309.
Edwards, J., Munson, B., & Beckman, M. E. (2011). Lexicon-phonology relationships and dynamics of early language development. Journal of Child Language, 38, 35-40.
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they can tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 102-134). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Estes, G., Evans, J. L., & Else-Quest, N. M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 177-195.
Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419-439.
Fowler, A. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to I. Y. Liberman (pp. 97-117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2001). Does strength of phonological representations predict phonological awareness in preschool children? Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 301-325.
Frisch, S. A., Large, N. R., & Pisoni, D. B. (2000). Perception of wordlikeness: Effects of segment probability and length on the processing of nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 481-496.
Gallon, N., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). Non-word repetition: An investigation of phonological complexity in children with Grammatical SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 435-455.
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513-543.
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A-M. (1994). Children’s phonological working memory: Contributions of long-term knowledge and rehearsal. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 672-688.
Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A-M., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Do young children rehearse? An individual difference analysis. Memory and Cognition, 22, 201-207.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200-213.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990a). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439-454.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990b). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336-360.
Gathercole, S. E., Frankish, C. R., Pickering, S. J., & Peaker, S. (1999). Phonotactic influences on short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 84-95.
Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., Service, E., & Martin, A. J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966-979.
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in children. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 387-406.
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349-367.
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental psychology, 28(5), 887.
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Baddeley, A. D, & Emslie, H. (1994). The children’s test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2, 103-127.new window
Girbau, D., & Schwartz, R. G. (2007). Non-word repetition in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 59-75.
Guiberson, M., & Rodriguez, B. L. (2013). Classification accuracy of nonword repetition when used with preschool-age Spanish-speaking children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 44, 121-132.
Gupta, P. (2009). A computational model of nonword repetition, immediate serial recall, and nonword learning. In A. Thorn & M. Page (Eds.), Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain (pp. 108-135). Hove & NY: Psychology Press.
Henry, L. A. (1991). Development of auditory memory span: The role of rehearsal. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 493-511.
Hoff, E., Core, C., & Bridges, K. (2008). Non-word repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20- to 24-month-olds. Journal of Child Language, 35, 903-916.
Hox, J. (2010). Multilevel analysis, techniques and applications. Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hsieh, Y-S. (2007). Nonword repetition in children with phonological disorders (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Taipei College of Nursing, Taipei, Taiwan.
Hsu, H. (2005). The association of phonological disorders and syntactic disorders: A study of Mandarin-speaking children (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.
Hsu, Joseph, H. (1987). A study of the stages of development and acquisition of Mandarin Chinese by children in Taiwan. Taipei: Crane.
Hu, C-F. (2004). The development of phonological representations among Chinese-speaking children. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 2, 103-130.
Hu, C. F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55-79.
Huang, R., Jian, H., Zhu, L., & Lu. L. (2011). Receptive and expressive vocabulary test. Taipei: Psychological Publishing.new window
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685-701.
Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C., & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 241-253.
Ingram. D. (1974). The relationship between comprehension and production. In R. Schiefelbusch & L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspective: Acquisition, retardation, and intervention (pp. 313-334). Baltimore, Md.: University Park Press.
Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition: Method, description and explanation. NY: Cambridge University Press.
Jeng, Heng-hsiung. (1985). A developmentalist view of child phonology. Studies in Language and Literature, 1, 1-29.

Jones, G, & Witherstone, H. L. (2010). Lexical and sublexical knowledge influences the encoding, storage, and articulation of nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 39, 588-599.
Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630-645.
Kovács, G., & Racsmány, M. (2008). Handling L2 input in phonological STM: The effect of non-L1 phonetic segments and non-L1 phonotactics on nonword repetition. Language Learning, 58, 597-624.
Lee, J. C. (2005). Nonword repetition and phonological processing in Mandarin-speaking preschool children (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1977). The acquisition of tone in Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 4, 185-199.
Li, N., & Cheung, H. (2014). Phonological analysis facilitates nonword repetition in young children. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 56, 359-378.
Lin, Grace Bao-Guey, & Lin, Mei-hsiu. (1994). A study on the development of language abilities in preschool children in Taiwan, R.O.C. Bulletin of Special Education, 10, 259-281.
Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22-38.
Liu, Y-C. (2007). Articulation development in final nasal consonants in 2- to 5-year-old Mandarin-speaking children (Unpublished master’s thesis). Taipei Municipal University of Education, Taipei, Taiwan.. c
Liu, H-M., & Tsao, F-M. (2010). Mandarin-Chinese Communicative Development Inventory (Taiwan). Taipei: Psychological Publishing.
Lu, L., & Liu, H. X. (1994). PPVT-revised, Chinese version. Taipei: Psychology.
Mann, V. A., & Foy, J. G. (2007). Speech development patterns and phonological awareness in preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 57, 51-74.
Marchman, V., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21, 339-366.new window
Maridaki-Kassotaki, Katerina. (2002). The relation between phonological memory skills and reading ability in Greek-speaking children: Can training of phonological memory contribute to reading development? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 17, 63-73.
Masoura, E. V., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005). Phonological short-term memory skills and new word learning in young Greek children. Memory, 13, 422-429
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lervåg, A., Lyster, S. H., Klem, M., Hagtvet, B. & Hulme, C. (2012). Nonword-repetition ability does not appear to be a causal influence on children’s vocabulary development. Psychological Science, 23, 1092-1098.
Melka, F. (1997). Receptive vs. productive aspects of vocabulary. In N. Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary description, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 84-102). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P. M., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 306-323.
Metsala, J. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Education Psychology, 91, 3-19.

Metsala, J., & Chisholm, G. M. (2010). The influence of lexical status and neighborhood density on children’s nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 489-506.
Metsala, J., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89-120). New York: Erlbaum.
Miyazaki, Y., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2000). Tests for linkage of multiple cohorts in an accelerated longitudinal design. Psychological Methods, 5, 44-63.
Morra, S., & Camba, R. (2009). Vocabulary learning in primary school children: Working memory and long-term memory components. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 156-178.
Munson, B. (2006). Nonword repetition and levels of abstraction in phonological knowledge. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 577-581.
Munson, B., Beckman, M., & Edwards, J. (2012). Abstraction and specificity in early lexical representations: Climbing the ladder of abstraction. In A. C. Cohn, C. Fougeron & M. Huffman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of laboratory phonology (pp. 288-309). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Munson, B., Kurtz, B.A., & Windsor, J. (2005). The Influence of Vocabulary Size, Phonotactic Probability, and Wordlikeness on Nonword Repetitions of Children with and without Language Impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1033-1047.
Nairne, J. S., Neath, I., & Serra, M. (1997). Proactive interference plays a role in the word-length effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, 541-545.
Nazzi, T. (2005). Use of phonetic specificity during the acquisition of new words: Differences between consonants and vowels. Cognition, 98, 13-30.
Nespor, M., Peña, M., & Mehler, J. (2003). On the different roles of vowels and consonants in speech processing and language acquisition. Lingue e Linguaggio, 2, 221-247.
Ramachandra, V., Hewitt, L. E., & Brackenbury, T. (2011). The relationship between phonological memory, phonological sensitivity, and incidental word learning. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40, 93-109.
Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2011). Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment and autism plus language impairments: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 23-36.
Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised (Leiter-R) manual. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting.
Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2- to 4-year-olds: Evidence from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 223-234.
Service, E., Maury, S., & Luotoniemi, E. (2007). Individual differences in phonological learning and verbal STM span. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1122-1135.
Shiu, Huei-shiou. (1990). The phonological acquisition by Mandarin-speaking children: A longitudinal case study on children from 9 months through three years old (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan.
Shriberg, L. D., Lohmeier, H. L., Campbell, T. F., Dollaghan, C. A., Green, J. R., & Moore, C. A. (2009). A nonword repetition task for speakers with misarticulations: The syllable repetition task (SRT). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 1189-1212.

Smith, A., Goffman, L., Sasisekaran, J., Weber-Fox, C. (2012). Language and motor abilities of preschool children who stutter: Evidence from behavioral and kinematic indices of nonword repetition performance. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 37, 344-358.
Smith, B. L., McGregor, K. K., & DeMille, D. (2006). Phonological development in lexically precocious 2-year-olds. Applied psycholinguistics, 27, 355-375.
Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369-373.
Sosa, A. V., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2006). Patterns of intra-word phonological variability during the second year of life. Journal of Child Language, 33, 31-50.
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38, 1-34.
Stokes, S. F., & Klee, T. (2009). The diagnostic accuracy of a new test of early nonword repetition differentiating late talking and typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 872-882.new window
Stokes, S. F., Wong, A. M-Y., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, L. B. (2006). Nonword repetition and sentence repetition as clinical markers of specific language impairment: The case of Cantonese. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 219-236.
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1321-1337.
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1987). The phoneme as a perceptumotor structure. In A. Allport, D. MacKay, W. Prinz, & E. Scheerer (Eds.), Language perception and production (pp. 67-84). London: Academic Press.
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (2000). Imitation and the emergence of segments. Phonetica, 57, 275-283.
Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Goodell, E. W. (1995). Gestures, features and segments in early child speech. In B. de Gelder, & J. Morais (Eds.), Speech and reading: A comparative approach (pp. 65-88). Hove, England: Erlbaum (UK), Taylor & Francis.
Taylor, L. J., Maybery, M. T., Grayndler, L., & Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2014). Evidence for distinct cognitive profiles in autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 19-30.
Towse, J. N., & Hitch, G. J. (1995). Is there a relationship between task demand and storage space in tests of working memory capacity? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 108-124.
Treiman, R., & Breaux, A. M. (1982). Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: Their use by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 569-598.
Tseng, Shu-Chuan. (2013). Lexical coverage in Taiwan Mandarin conversation. International Journal for Computational Linguistics and Chinese Language Processing, 18, 1-18.
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1998). When words compete: Levels of processing in spoken word perception. Psychological Science, 9, 325-329.
Vitevitch, M. S. & Luce, P. A. (2005). Increases in phonotactic probability facilitate spoken nonword repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 193-204.
Walley, A. C. (1993). The role of vocabulary growth in children’s spoken word recognition and segmentalization ability. Developmental Review, 13, 286-350.
Wang, F. N-M., Huang, Helen, Chen, Ching-Wen, Phillips, Penny. (1986). Articulation development in Mandarin-speaking preschool children ages 3-6 in Taiwan. Chang Gung Medical Journal, 9, 68-75.
Weill, France. (2011). The role of verbal working memory in new word learning in toddlers 24 to 30 months old (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Seton Hall University, New Jersey, US.
Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Lobitz, K. F., & Pham, G. T. (2010). Cross-language nonword repetition by bilingual and monolingual children. American Journal of Speech-language Pathology, 19, 298-301.
Yang, Shu-wen. (2002). The role of phonological capacity in children’s vocabulary learning process (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.
Yuen, P. J. (2009). The development of phonological memory and fast-mapping in preschool children (Unpublished master’s thesis). The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
Zhang, X. (2011). The phonological processing skills in predicting word recognition in children with specific language impairment (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Taipei University of Nursing and Health Science, Taipei, Taiwan.
Zhu, H. (2006). The normal and disordered phonology of Putonghua (modern standard Chinese)-speaking children. In H. Zhu & B. Dodd (Eds.), Phonological development and disorders in children: A multilingual perspective (pp. 81-108). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Zhu, H., & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (modern standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language, 27, 3-42.
錡寶香(2009)。兒童語言與溝通發展。台北市:心理出版社。new window


 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
:::
無相關著作
 
QR Code
QRCODE