:::

詳目顯示

回上一頁
題名:正常老年人在心智理論、執行功能與社會智力關係的探討
作者:葉在庭 引用關係
作者(外文):Zai-Ting Yeh
校院名稱:國立臺灣大學
系所名稱:心理學研究所
指導教授:花茂棽
學位類別:博士
出版日期:2007
主題關鍵詞:心智理論正常老年人執行功能社會智力Theory of mindnormal agingexecutive functionssocial intelligences.
原始連結:連回原系統網址new window
相關次數:
  • 被引用次數被引用次數:期刊(1) 博士論文(0) 專書(0) 專書論文(0)
  • 排除自我引用排除自我引用:0
  • 共同引用共同引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:66
個體具有預測行動者心中的想法、感受、需求與信念的能力,稱之為「心智理論」,在神經科學方面的研究顯示心智理論不受其他認知功能如語言、智力、記憶及執行功能的影響,研究認為大腦內腹側額葉在心智理論的運作扮演重要的角色,而且與人類的社會行為有關。雖然過去有關心智理論的研究大部分均集中在兒童發展階段,但是近年來,已經開始對老年人在心智理論作業的表現有所探討,然而此部分的結果卻呈現相當大的差異。本研究的目的在於 (1) 以具有信效度且適合國內版本的心智理論題目評估正常老年人在心智理論作業的表現是否有退化現象? (2) 探討正常老年人心智理論與執行功能的關係?及 (3) 正常老年人的心智理論能力是否能預測其社會智力?以38位中老年人(平均年齡為70.55),32位老年人(平均年齡為80.13)及在教育程度、語文理解指標、記憶能力及MMSE分數均一致的38位年輕人(平均年齡為22.77)進行心智理論、執行功能及社會智力量表的評估,在中老年及老年組均以臨床神經心理測驗排除了罹患失智症的可能性後,以共變數分析排除社會地位指數或執行功能影響,結果顯示不論中老年組或老年組在心智理論作業的表現與年輕組沒有差異,而執行功能則隨著年齡有退化的趨勢,支持大腦背外側前額葉與內腹側前額葉有不同的退化速度。此外,本研究以結構方程模式進行心智理論、執行功能與社會智力間的預測模式分析,發現心智理論可以預測老年人的社會智力,而非執行功能。此外,心智理論能力並未因執行功能的退化而受到影響,因此,本論文支持執行功能在心智理論的發展過程扮演建構性的角色。
Theory of Mind (ToM) refers to the ability to represent mental states, such as beliefs, and intentions, and is distinct from the ability to represent ‘real’ states of affairs. It enables us to make attribution and to reason about mental states and, in doing so, to understand and predict the behavior of other people. Researches have suggested that Theory of Mind may represent a dissociable, modular brain system that is related to, but separable, from other brain functions including memory, language and executive functions. Studies of neuroimaging provide further evidence that the ventromedial regions of the prefrontal cortex are associated with ToM. Although the majority of research in ToM focused on young children or individuals with autism, only some recent studies have investigated ToM throughout the lifespan, especially in normal aging. However, the results have been controversial. The specific aims of the present study were to: (1) examine whether healthy elderly people are impaired on those tasks including the well-established ToM ones, (2) explore the relationship between ToM and executive functions in elder people, and (3) examine whether social intelligence is correlated more with ToM than with executive functions. Thirty-eight young-old (mean age = 70.55), thirty-two old-old (mean age = 80.13) and thirty-eight young (mean age = 22.77) age groups, matched on education, memory function and MMSE scores, and each subject received well-established ToM tasks, executive function tests and social intelligence scales. The results revealed that there were no age effects on ToM tasks even after taking account of measures of social-economic status and executive function. However, executive functions were impaired both in the young-old and old-old groups when compared with the young group. ToM performances, but not executive function, were strongly correlated with social intelligence among elderly individuals. Base on the present results, it is thus suggested that ToM might play an important role in social intelligence.
王佳玉(2003)。「阿茲海默型失智症患者之自我覺知功能」(未發表之碩士論文)。台南:國立成功大學行為醫學研究所碩士論文。
花茂棽、張本聖、林克能、楊建銘、盧小蓉、陳心怡 (2005)。「魏氏記憶量表第三版(WMS-III)中文版」。台北:中國行為科學社股份有限公司。
駱重鳴(1983)。「生活壓力、適應方式與身心健康」(未發表之碩士論文)。台北:國立台灣大學心理學研究所碩士論文。
陳榮華、陳心怡(2004)。「魏氏成人智力量表第三版(WAIS-III)中文版」。台北:中國行為科學社股份有限公司。
黃毅志(2003)。「台灣地區新職業聲望與社經地位量表」之建構與評估:社會科學與教育社會學研究本土化」。教育研究集刊,49,1-31。new window
Abell, F., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15, 1-16.
Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 469-479.
Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 1032-1037.
Bachman, D. L., Wolf, P. A., Linn, R., Knoefel, J. E., Cobb, J., & Belanger, A.(1992). Prevalence of dementia and probable senile dementia of the Alzheimer type in the Framingham Study. Neurology, 42, 115-119.
Bauminger, N. (2006). Brief Report: Group Social-Multimodal Intervention for HFASD. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 41, 120-127.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The descent of mind : psychological perspectives on hominid evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child''s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285-297.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ''theory of mind''? Cognition, 21, 37-46.
Baron-Cohen, S., O''Riordan, M., Stone, V. E., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism Development Disorder, 29, 407-418.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Moriarty, J., & Schmitz, B. (1994). Recognition of mental state terms: Clinical findings in children with autism and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 640-649.
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7-15.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293-1295.
Bennett, J. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Commentary on D. Premack & G. Woodruff, Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 558-560.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fix indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246.
Bentler, P. M., & Yuan, K.-H. (1999). Structural equation modeling with small samples: Test statistics. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 34, 181-197.
Bibby, H., & McDonald, S. (2005). Theory of mind after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 43, 99-114.
Biehl, M., Matsumoto, D., Ekman, P., & Hearn, V. (1997). Matsumoto and Ekman''s Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE): Reliability data and cross-national differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 3-21.
Bird, C. M., Castelli, F., Malik, O., Frith, U., & Husain, M. (2004). The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on theory of Mind and cognition. Brain, 127, 914-928.
Boone, K. B., Miller, B. L., Lesser, I. M., & Hill, E. (1990). Performance on frontal lobe tests in healthy, older individuals. Developmental Neuropsychology, 6, 215-223.
Bosacki, S. & Astington, J. W. (1999). Theory of mind in preadolescence: Relations between social understanding and social competence. Social Development, 8, 237-255.
Brayne, C., Gill, C., Huppert, F. A., Barkley, C., Gehlhaar, E., Girling, D. M. (1995). Incidence of clinically diagnosed subtypes of dementia in an elderly population. Cambridge Project for Later Life. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 255-262.
Brinkman, S. D., Largen, J. W., Gerganoff, S., & Pomara, N. (1983). Russell''s Revised Wechsler Memory Scale in the evaluation of dementia. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 989-993.
Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M. C., & Decety, J. (2000). A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Neuroimage, 11, 157-166.
Caltagirone, C., Carlesimo, A., Nocentini, U., & Vicari, S. (1989). Defective concept formation in Parkinsonians is independent from mental deterioration. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 52, 334-337.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 73-92.
Capage, L. & Watson, A. C. (2001). Individual differences in theory of mind, aggressive behavior, and social skills in young children. Early Education & Development, 12, 613-628.
Charman, T., Carroll, F., & Sturge, C. (2001). Theory of mind, executive function and social competence in boys with ADHD. Emotional & Behavioural Difficulties, 6, 31-49.
Cicone, M., Wapner, W., & Gardner, H. (1980). Sensitivity to emotional expressions and situations in organic patients. Cortex, 16, 145-158.
Coffey, C. E., Wilkinson, W. E., Weiner, R. D., Parashos, I. A., Djang, W. T. & Webb, M. C. (1993). Quantitative cerebral anatomy in depression. A controlled magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 7-16.
Coleman, P. D., & Flood, D. G. (1987). Neuron numbers and dendritic extent in normal aging and Alzheimer''s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 8, 521-545.
Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Keil, K., & Haxby, J. V. (1997). Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory. Nature, 386(6625), 608-611.
Cullum, C.M., Thompson, L.L., & Smernoff, E.N. (1993). Three-word recall as a measure of memory. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 321-329.
Daigneault, S., Braun, C. M., & Whitaker, H. A. (1992). Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non-perseverative prefrontal measures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 99-114.
Damasio, A.R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Serier B, Biological Science, 351, 1413-1420.
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behavioural Brain Research, 41, 81-94.
Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 568-570.
Double, K. L., Halliday, G. M., Kril, J. J., Harasty, J. A., Cullen, K. & Brooks, W. S. (1996). Topography of brain atrophy during normal aging and Alzheimer''s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 17, 513-521.
Fine, C., Lumsden, J., & Blair, R. (2001). Dissociation between "theory of mind" and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain, 124, 287-298.
Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1995). Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of "theory of mind" in story comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109-128.
Fristoe, N. M., Salthouse, T. A., & Woodard, J. L. (1997). Examination of age-related deficits on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuropsychology, 11, 428-436.
Frye, D. (1999). Development of intention: The relation of executive function to theory of mind. In P. Zelazo, J. Astington & D.R.. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control. (pp. 119-132). Mahwah (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Press.
Frye, D. (2000). Theory of mind, domain specificity, and reasoning.In P. Mitchell & K.J. Riggs (Eds.), Children''s reasoning and the mind. (pp. 149-167). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., Brooks, P. J., & Samuels, M. C. (1996). Inference and action in early causal reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 120-131.
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483-527.
Gallagher, H., Happe, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ''theory of the mind'' in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 11-21.
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ''theory of mind''. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 77-83.
Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition and the development of the theory of mind. Mind & Language, 16, 494-541.
Goel, V., Grafman, J., Sadato, N., & Hallett, M. (1995). Modeling other minds. Neuroreport, 6, 1741-1746.
Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children''s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearanceeality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26-37.
Gregory, C., Lough, S., Stone, V., Erzinclioglu, S., Martin, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer''s disease: Theoretical and practical implications. Brain, 125, 752-764.
Gregory, C. A., & Hodges, J. R. (1996). Frontotemporal dementia: Use of consensus criteria and prevalence of psychiatric features. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 9, 145-153.
Guajardo, N. R., & Watson, A. C. (2002). Narrative discourse and theory of mind development. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163, 305-325.
Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2000). The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: a quantitative review. Neuropsychology, 14, 224-232.
Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Obrist, W. D., Skolnick, B. E., & Reivich, M. (1987). Age and regional cerebral blood flow at rest and during cognitive activity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 617-621.
Happe, F. (1994). An Advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters''s thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 24, 129-154.new window
Happe, F., Malhi, G. S., & Checkley, S. (2001). Acquired mind-blindness following frontal lobe surgery? A single case study of impaired ''theory of mind'' in a patient treated with stereotactic anterior capsulotomy. Neuropsychologia, 39, 83-90.
Happe, F. G. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101-119.new window
Happe, F. G., Winner, E., & Brownell, H. (1998). The getting of wisdom: theory of mind in old age. Developmental Psychology, 34, 358-362.
Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron bar through the head. Boston Medical and Surgical Journal, 39, 389-393.
Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The Awkward Moments Test: A naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 30, 225-236.
Hua, M.-S.,Chang, S.-H., & Chen, S.-T. (1997). Factor structure and age effects with an aphasia test battery in normal Taiwanese adults. Neuropsychology, 11, 156-162.new window
Hughes, C. (1998a). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233-253.
Hughes, C. (1998b). Finding your marbles: Does preschoolers'' strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind? Developmental Psychology, 34, 1326-1339.
Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child & Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131-142.
Johnson, C. N., & Wellman, H. M. (1980). Children''s developing understanding of mental verbs: Remember, know, and guess. Child Development, 51, 1095-1102.
Johnson, J. A., Cheek, J. M., & Smither, R. (1983). The structure of empathy. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 45, 1299-1312.
Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new "advanced" test of theory of mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 43, 517-528.new window
Kaplan, J. A., Brownell, H. H., Jacobs, J. R., & Gardner, H. (1990). The effects of right hemisphere damage on the pragmatic interpretation of conversational remarks. Brain & Language, 38, 315-333.
Kokmen, E., Beard, C. M., O''Brien, P. C., Offord, K. P., & Kurland, L. T. (1993). Is the incidence of dementing illness changing? A 25-year time trend study in Rochester, Minnesota (1960-1984). Neurology, 43, 1887-1892.
Kononova, E. P. (1962). The frontal region of the brain: Leningrad medgiz Press.
Lalonde, C. E & Chandler, M. J. (1995). False belief understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition & Emotion, 9,167-185.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." Psychological Review, 94, 412-426.
Leslie, A. M., & Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Two conjectures. Developmental Science, 1, 247-253.
Letenneur, L., Commenges, D., Dartigues, J. F., & Barberger-Gateau, P. (1994). Incidence of dementia and Alzheimer''s disease in elderly community residents of south-western France. International Journal of Epidemiology, 23, 1256-1261.
Libon, D. J., Glosser, G., Malamut, B. L., & Kaplan, E. (1994). Age, executive functions, and visuospatial functioning in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology, 8, 38-43.
Liu, C. K., Lai, C. L., Tai, C. T., Lin, R. T., Yen, Y. Y., & Howng, S. L. (1998). Incidence and subtypes of dementia in southern Taiwan: impact of socio-demographic factors. Neurology, 50, 1572-1579.
Liu, H. C., Fuh, J. L., Wang, S. J., Liu, C. Y., Larson, E. B., & Lin, K. N. (1998). Prevalence and subtypes of dementia in a rural Chinese population. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 12, 127-134.
Liu, H. C., Lin, K. N., Teng, E. L., Wang, S. J., Fuh, J. L., & Guo, N. W. (1995). Prevalence and subtypes of dementia in Taiwan: a community survey of 5297 individuals. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 43, 144-149.
Loddo, S. (2000). "Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism": Comment. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 30, 622-623.
Logue, P., & Wyrick, L. (1979). Initial validation of Russell''s Revised Wechsler Memory Scale: A comparison of normal aging versus dementia. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 47, 176-178.
Lough, S., Gregory, C., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). Dissociation of social cognition and executive function in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase, 7, 123-130.
Lund, & Manchester Groups. (1994). Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 57, 416-418.
MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: A dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology & Aging, 17, 598-609.
Masterman, D. L., & Cummings, J. L. (1997). Frontal-subcortical circuits: the anatomic basis of executive, social and motivated behaviors. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 11, 107-114.
Maylor, E. A., Moulson, J. M., Muncer, A. M., & Taylor, L. A. (2002). Does performance on theory of mind tasks decline in old age? British Journal of Psychology, 93, 465-485.
McKinnon, M. C.& Moscovitch, M. (2007). Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: Theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. Cognition, 102, 179-218.
Morrison, J. H., & Hof, P. R. (1997). Life and death of neurons in the aging brain. Science, 278, 412-419.
Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1995). Frontal lobes, memory, and aging. In: Grafman J, Holyoak K, & Boller F (Eds.), Structure and functions of the human prefrontal cortex (pp.411). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
Mundy, P., & Neal, A. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. San Diego: US: Academic Press.
Mundy, P., & Stella, J. (2000). Joint attention, social orienting, and nonverbal communication in autism. Wetherby, Amy M, (Ed); Prizant, 422pp.
Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., & Gustafson, L. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical daignostic criteria. Neurology, 51, 1546-1554.
Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex, 12, 313-324.new window
Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior: Center for Human Information Processing [Techinal Report No.99].
Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 32, 1081-1105.
Pandya, D. N., & Yeterian, E. H. (1996). Comparison of prefrontal architecture and connections. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London - Series B: Biological Sciences, 351(1346), 1423-1432.
Parks, R. W.& Levine, D. S. (1998). Neural network modeling of Wisconsin Card Sorting and verbal fluency tests: Applications with frontal lobe-damaged and Alzheimer''s disease patients. In R.W. Parks & D.S.Levine (Eds.), Fundamentals of neural network modeling: Neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience. (pp. 357-380). MIT Press.
Paulus, M. P. & Frank, L. R. (2003). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation is critical for preference judgments. Neuroreport, 14, 1311-1315.
Perner, J., Kain, W., & Barchfeld, P. (2002). Executive control and higher-order theory of mind in children at risk of ADHD. Infant & Child Development, 11, 141-158.
Perner, J., Stummer, S., & Lang, B. (1999). Executive functions and theory of mind: Cognitive complexity or functional dependence? In P. Zelazo, J. Astington & D.R.. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control. (pp. 133-145). Mahwah (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Press.
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). " John thinks that Mary thinks that...". Attribution of second-order false beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437-471.
Philip, N. & Barbara,N. (1991). Development through life—a psychological approach. Thomoson International Publishing.
Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1997). Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven''s Progressive Matrices Test. . Cognitive Psychology, 33, 43-63.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515-526.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1978). When is attribution of beliefs justified? Commentary on D. Premack & G. Woodruff, Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 592-593.
Ravnkilde, B., Videbech, P., Rosenberg, R., Gjedde, A. & Gade, A. (2002). Putative tests of frontal lobe function: A PET-study of brain activation during Stroop''s Test and verbal fluency. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 534-547.new window
Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance:Integration of structural and functional findings. Mahwah,New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Head, D., Dupuis, J. H., & Acker, J. D. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive aging: evidence from structural magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychology, 12, 95-114.
Roncone, R.,Falloon, I. R.H., Mazza, M., De-Risio, A., Pollice, R., & Necozione, S. (2002). Is theory of mind in schizophrenia more strongly associated with clinical and social functioning than with neurocognitive deficits? Psychopathology, 35, 280-288.
Rolls, E. T. (1996). The orbitofrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London - Series B: Biological Sciences, 351, 1433-1443.
Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 284-294.
Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55, 11-29.
Rowe, A. D., Bullock, P. R., Polkey, C. E., & Morris, R. G. (2001). ''Theory of mind'' impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. Brain, 124, 600-616.
Russell, J. (1997). How executive disorders can bring about an inadequate ''theory of mind''. Autism as an executive disorder, Oxford University Press.
Saltzman, J., Strauss, E., Hunter, M., & Archibald, S. (2000). Theory of mind and executive functions in normal human aging and Parkinson''s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 781-788.
Sanfey, A. G., Hastie, R., Colvin, M. K. & Grafman, J. (2003). Phineas gauged: Decision-making and the human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1218-1229.
Shafer, A. B. (1999). Relation of the big five and factor V subcomponents to social intelligence. European Journal of Personality, 13, 225-240.
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1991). Higher-order cognitive impairments and frontal lobe lesions in man. Frontal lobe function and dysfunction, Oxford University Press
Shamary-Tsoory, S., Tomer, R., Berger, B., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2003). Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain damage: The role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 324-337.
Shaw, P., Lawrence, E. J., Radbourne, C., Bramham, J., Polkey, C. E., & David, A. S. (2004). The impact of early and late damage to the human amygdala on ''theory of mind'' reasoning. Brain, 127, 1535-1548.
Shaw, T. G., Mortel, K. F., Meyer, J. S., Rogers, R. L., Hardenberg, J., & Cutaia, M. M. (1984). Cerebral blood flow changes in benign aging and cerebrovascular disease. Neurology, 34, 855-862.
Siegal, M., Carrington, J., & Radel, M. (1996). Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain & Language, 53, 40-50.
Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2002). Neural systems involved in "theory of mind". Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 463-471.
Silvera, D. H., Martinussen, M., & Dahl, T. (2001). The Tromso Social Intelligence Scale, a self-report measure of social intelligence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 313-319.
Steerneman, P., Jackson, S., Pelzer, H., & Muris, P. (1996). Children with social handicaps: An intervention programme using a theory of mind approach. Clinical Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 1, 251-563.
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640-656.
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J. & Young, A. (2003). Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia. 41, 209-220.
Stuss, D., Gallup, G. G. J., & Alexander, M. P. (2001). The frontal lobes are necessary for "theory of mind". Brain, 124, 279-286.
Stuss, D. T., & Anderson, V. (2004). The frontal lobes and theory of mind: developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research, Brain and Cognition, 55 , 69-83.
Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 167-177.
Tranel, D. (2002) Emotion, decision making, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. In D.T. Stuss & R.T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp.338-352). London: Oxford University Press.
West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272-292.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs:Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children''s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.
Shyu Y.I., & Yip P.K. (2001). Factor structure and explanatory variables of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for elderly persons in Taiwan. Journal of the Formosan Medical Association. 100, 676-683.
Zee, K. V. D., Thijs, M., & Schakel, A. L. (2002). The relationship of emotional intelligence with academic intelligence and the big five. European Journal of Personality, 16, 103-125.
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
:::
無相關著作
 
QR Code
QRCODE